Another dominant, albeit controversial (e.g., Amitay, Ben-Yehudah, Banai, & Ahissar,
2002; Olulade, Napoliello, & Eden,
2013; Sperling, Lu, Manis, & Seidenberg,
2005) theory is known as the magnocellular–dorsal (M–D) theory of DD (Livingstone, Rosen, Drislane, & Galaburda,
1991; Stein & Walsh,
1997), which stems from the observation that a high percentage of reading disabled children are impaired in the specific visual M–D pathway (see Boden & Giaschi,
2007; Facoetti,
2012; Gori & Facoetti,
2014; Stein & Walsh,
1997; Vidyasagar & Pammer,
2010, for reviews). The M–D pathway originates in the ganglion cells of the retina, passes through the M-layer of the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN), and finally reaches the occipital and parietal cortices (Maunsell & Newsome,
1987). The M–D stream is considered blind to colors and responds optimally to contrast differences, low spatial frequencies, high temporal frequencies, and both real and illusory motion (e.g., Gori, Giora, & Stubbs,
2010; Gori, Giora, Yazdanbakhsh, & Mingolla,
2011; Gori, Hamburger, & Spillmann,
2006; Gori & Yazdanbakhsh,
2008; Livingstone & Hubel,
1987; Morrone et al.,
2000; Ruzzoli et al.,
2011; Yazdanbakhsh & Gori,
2011), which is also, surprisingly, perceived by animals without a cortex, such as fish (Gori, Agrillo, Dadda, & Bisazza,
2014a). Individuals with DD are less sensitive than typically reading controls to luminance patterns and motion displays with high temporal and low spatial frequencies (e.g., Eden et al.,
1996), visual features that are known to be associated with the M–D pathway. However, they perform similarly to the controls on tasks preferentially associated with the parvocellular–ventral pathway (Gori et al.,
2014b), such as those involving color and form (Merigan & Maunsell,
1993). The M–D theory can also be embedded in its multisensory (i.e., visual and auditory) version, called the temporal processing hypothesis, which suggests that children with DD have specific deficits in processing rapidly presented sensory stimuli in either the visual or auditory modalities (see Farmer & Klein,
1995; Hari & Renvall,
2001, for reviews). Importantly, the M–D temporal hypothesis explicitly claims that phonological decoding deficits in individuals with DD could arise from impairments in dynamic sensory processing of visual and auditory stimuli (e.g., Facoetti et al.,
2010b; Gori et al.,
2014b; Ruffino et al.,
2010,
2014). It has been reported that up to 75% of dyslexic individuals show visual temporal processing deficits (Lovegrove, Martin, & Slaghuis,
1986). Moreover, a postmortem study showed that in the brain of individuals with dyslexia the M neurons of the LGN were significantly smaller than those found in normal readers' brains, and the P neurons did not differ between the two groups (Livingstone et al.,
1991). This study recently received strong support from the first in vivo study (Giraldo-Chica, Hegarty, & Schneider,
in press) showing smaller LGN volume in a larger sample of individuals with DD compared to controls. Recently,
Gori et al. (2014b) and Gori et al. (
in press) demonstrated, for the first time, that children with DD showed a lower performance in both a task that taps the M (i.e., spatial frequency doubling illusion; Kelly,
1966) and one that taps the D (i.e., rotating tilted lines illusion, Gori & Hamburger,
2006; Gori & Yazdanbakhsh,
2008; Yazdanbakhsh & Gori,
2008, and the accordion grating, Gori et al.,
2011; Gori, Giora, Yazdanbakhsh, & Mingolla,
2013; Yazdanbakhsh & Gori,
2011) portion of the M-D pathway, not only in comparison with an age-matched control group, but also with a RL control group. Some longitudinal studies provided strong evidence in the direction of a causal link between a prereading M–D deficit and future reading acquisition (e.g., Boets, Vandermosten, Cornelissen, Wouters, & Ghesquière,
2011; Boets, Wouters, van Wieringen, De Smedt, & Ghesquière,
2008; Kevan & Pammer,
2008;
2009). These studies supported the hypothesis that the M–D deficit is not caused by lack of reading abilities (effect of DD) but should be considered a core deficit of DD. Gori et al. (
in press) also showed the first reported association between a genetic variance (the DCDC2-Intron deletion) and an M–D deficit in both individuals with DD and typical readers. The DCDC2-Intron deletion is a proved DD genetic risk factor (e.g., Marino et al.,
2011; Marino et al.,
2012; Marino et al.,
2014; Mascheretti et al.,
2013; Mascheretti et al.,
in press; Meng et al.,
2005; Riva, Marino, Giorda, Molteni, & Nobile,
in press). According to recent studies, the M–D pathway also seems to be specifically involved in audiovisual detection enhancements (e.g., Harrar et al.,
2014; Pérez-Bellido, Soto-Faraco, & Lopez-Moliner,
2013), suggesting an additional causal link between the M–D deficit and the basic cross-modal integration dysfunction in individuals with DD. Interestingly, the M–D deficit in individuals with DD was found also in logographic languages, such as Chinese (e.g., Zhao, Qian, Bi, & Coltheart,
2014). Gori and Facoetti (
2014) recently stressed the importance of showing the positive effects of a rehabilitation approach based on an M–D stream deficit. If an M–D stream deficit is really a cause of DD, it is expected that specific M–D stream training would be able to improve not only M–D functioning, but also reading abilities in individuals with DD.