Cortical magnification was calculated from error on a positional localization task that required central fixation while stimuli were localized parafoveally. Subjects were told to maintain fixation at all times, but eye movements were not measured. In fact, it was clear that strabismic subjects fixated away from the true location of the fixation cross with their amblyopic eye, in the direction consistent with their angle of squint (e.g.,
Figure 2). Furthermore, fixation of the amblyopic group was almost certainly more unstable than that of the normal group (Schor & Levi,
1980; González, Wong, Niechwiej-Szwedo, Tarita-Nistor, & Steinbach,
2012). We suggest that the absence of eye movement measures and the differences in fixational stability between groups are not problematic for the main findings of this study. First, positional error increased from the center to the periphery for all subjects, as would be expected if subjects were accurately fixating. Second, any constant error introduced by eccentric fixation was removed from the data prior to the analyses. Third, normal variation in fixational stability as well as pronounced nystagmus (e.g., in albinism and in rod-monochromats), does not affect the accuracy of retinotopic mapping with techniques that require steady central fixation (e.g., fMRI; Baseler et al.,
2002; Hoffmann, Tolhurst, Moore, & Morland,
2003; Crossland, Morland, Feely, von dem Hagen, & Rubin,
2008). For differences in fixational stability or attention to have produced the results shown here, such instability or shifts in attention would have to be disproportionately larger for centrally presented probes than for peripherally presented probes, which is inconsistent with the properties of the task: In this task, fixational or attentional shifts, if any, would be greater at large (and not small) eccentricities, when judgments were made across a wider spatial extent, producing more error in the periphery and not the center. Even if subjects' fixation alternated between probes, the positional judgment would ultimately be made with reference to a peripheral point. For the above reasons, we suggest that eye movements, and attentional or fixational differences between groups, do not explain the results. The amblyopic group was on average, 12 years older than the normal group, introducing age as another possible confound (although both groups can be considered relatively young: mean 38 years vs. 26 years). Older subjects are reported to show less cortical activation than younger subjects (Crossland et al.,
2008), but age is not known to affect the cortical magnification function (see Conner, Sharma, Lemieux, & Mendola,
2004, for children vs. adults), or to produce a selective central visual field impairment (except in macular degeneration, which the observers in this study did not have). To examine any such effects of age, we conducted an analysis of covariance (ANCOVA), with
a as the dependent measure, Age as a covariate and Group as a between-subjects factor. The analysis showed a significant main effect of Age,
F(1, 46) = 5.80,
p = 0.02, and a significant main effect of Group,
F(1, 46) = 11.03,
p = 0.001, confirming that age was correlated with the dependent measure, but that the group difference remained significant after controlling for the effect of age. The homogeneity of slopes assumption was confirmed by the absence of a significant interaction between Group and Age,
F(1, 46) = 2.80,
p = 0.15. Hence, age does not account for the difference in the estimate of cortical magnification between the normally sighted and amblyopic subjects.