A puzzling claim made by McIntosh and Buonocore (
2014) is that the term remote distractor effect may no longer be a useful concept. This, they argue, is because ipsilateral distractors (at the target location) can produce inhibitory effects “and we have recently shown that these inhibitory effects can be even stronger than those of remote distractors (Buonocore & McIntosh,
2012).” In their discussion of ipsilateral distractor effects McIntosh and Buonocore do not make clear the important distinction between remote, and near, ipsilateral distractors, which is conflated in their study by the use of large rectangular distractors presented at the target location. Buonocore and McIntosh (
2012) examined the influence of contralateral and ipsilateral distractors, presented at the same eccentricity (5°) as the saccade target. The rectangular distractors varied in size from 1°–16° vertically and appeared after long (fixed) SOAs of either 120–130 ms after the target. With this configuration distractors greater than 3.5° vertically would fall both inside and outside the 20° spatial window of the RDE (Walker, Deubel, Schneider, & Findlay,
1997). Thus, distractor size and spatial relationship to the saccade target were conflated, and this could account for the nonlinear relationship they report. In their experiment 2 a weak SI inhibitory effect was observed with ipsilateral distractors that increased with distractor size (consistent with the spatial modulation of the RDE). The finding of a stronger ipsilateral distractor effect was observed only with the larger distractors as they note: “Ipsilateral events are more distracting than contralateral events, at least at larger distractor sizes > 4°” (Buonocore & McIntosh,
2012). Critically for McIntosh and Buonocore's argument a small SI effect (a decrease in saccade frequency) was observed with small 2° distractors, and the effect was similar in magnitude to that produced by contralateral distractors at the same eccentricity (see Buonocore & McIntosh,
2012, figures 2C and 3C). The finding that a distractor, appearing at the target location, can induce SI contrasts with the findings of Edelman and Xu (
2009) who presented small ipsilateral distractors on an axis 22° from the horizontal (thus outside RDE window) or at the location of the saccade goal (inside RDE window) in a memory-guided paradigm. Only distractors at 22° from the target axis produced an SI effect, while those presented at the saccade goal did not (an increase in the frequency of short latency express saccades was observed). Similarly Bompas and Sumner (
2011) reported no evidence of SI with small ipsilateral distractors at the target location (“we found virtually no effect of late distractors appearing at the location of the target,” p. 12509). Furthermore, the presence of SI with a distractor at the target location is incompatible with the neurophysiological explanations of both SI and the RDE (Bompas & Sumner,
2011; Buonocore & McIntosh,
2008,
2012; Casteau & Vitu,
2012; Edelman & Xu,
2009; Reingold & Stampe,
2002; Walker et al.,
1997; Walker, Kentridge, & Findlay,
1995). To summarize, studies using small ipsilateral distractors presented within the RDE spatial window have not revealed evidence of inhibition on measures of SI (Bompas & Sumner,
2011; Edelman & Xu,
2009) or SRT. The stronger SI effect observed by Buonocore and McIntosh (
2012) was for the larger ipsilateral distractors and this is not evidence against the remote distractor effect. Although McIntosh and Buonocore (
2014) state they prefer simple explanations to account for SDEs their own account has included additional endogenous attention components, for example: “Ipsilateral events are more distracting than contralateral events, at least at larger distractor sizes (>4). A plausible account of this size difference would be that endogenous attention allows strong top-down inhibition of the distractor-related activation, provided that the distractor is spatially removed from the target” (Buonocore & McIntosh,
2012, p. 38). The inclusion of an “endogenous attention” mechanism in addition to SI (which is regarded as an automatic inhibitory effect) raises additional questions about the interpretation of behavioral effects.