Whereas knowledge of the speed of objects in the environment is of critical importance, there is still no consensus on the nature of the processes that underlie the encoding of speed in the human visual system. Much of the work that addresses this problem has looked to biases in our perception of speed to inform both formal and informal models of speed encoding. Human speed perception has been shown to be readily influenced by the contrast of the scene viewed (e.g., Hammett, Champion, Thompson, & Morland,
2007; Thompson,
1982; Thompson, Brooks, & Hammett,
2006); and it is now well established that, at slow speeds, low contrast stimuli appear to move more slowly than their higher contrast analogues but, conversely, they can appear to move more quickly at higher speeds (> 8 Hz)
1 (e.g., Thompson,
1982; Thompson et al.,
2006). For convenience and following others (e.g., Brooks,
2001; Snowden, Stimpson, & Ruddle,
1998), we will refer to these biases in perceived speed as the Thompson Effect. This observation and others has led Thompson (
1982) and many others (e.g., Adelson & Bergen,
1986; Hammett, Thompson, & Bedingham,
2000; Harris,
1980; Smith & Edgar,
1994; Tolhurst, Sharpe, & Hart,
1973) to the suggestion that speed may be encoded as the ratio of two mechanisms tuned to low and high temporal frequencies (or “slow” and “fast” mechanisms) (
Figure 1, left hand panel). Whereas the physiological substrate of these mechanisms is not known, one clear candidate may be the subpopulations of Magno and Parvocellular cells (hereafter referred to as M and P cells, respectively) in the primate lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) (De Valois, Cottaris, Mahon, Elfar, & Wilson,
2000). The logic of this ratio class of model rests upon the assumption that speed is encoded as the relative activity of “slow” and “fast” mechanisms—at slow speeds, reducing contrast has proportionately less effect upon the response of the “slow” mechanism (since it is most sensitive to slower stimuli) and thus patterns appear slower. Similarly, at fast speeds, reducing contrast will have proportionately less effect upon the “fast” mechanism and will thus result in a perceptual speeding up. The ratio model can therefore adequately account for the Thompson Effect and other perceptual biases in speed such as those induced by changes in luminance and adaptive state (Hammett, Champion, Morland, & Thompson,
2005; Hammett et al.,
2007; Thompson,
1981).