To control for outliers, a procedure based on the one suggested by Chang, Tzeng, Hung, and Wu (
2011) was applied. At first, for each participant, all reproduced durations that were more than ±2
SDs from that participant's MRD for a given target interval were considered invalid trials and, thus, not included in further data analysis. By using this criterion, less than 4% of all trials were removed from data analysis. In a next step, each participant's remaining reproduced durations were submitted to a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with target intervals (800, 1000, and 1200 ms) as three levels of a repeated-measurement factor. The lack of a significant main effect of target duration on reproduced durations would imply an individual's inability to follow the instruction to reproduce the target intervals. None of our participants had to be excluded on the basis of this latter criterion.
Analysis of error rates on the two versions of the secondary task yielded faultless performance and, thus, indicated that all participants conformed to the instructions of the salience and control condition, respectively. There also was no indication of a statistically significant effect of stimulus shape (circles and squares) on MRD. Therefore, for further statistical analyses, data were collapsed across shapes of stimuli. Means and standard deviations for reproduced durations as a function of target duration, nontemporal stimulus size, stimulus salience, and manipulation interval are given in
Table 1.
In a next step, a four-way ANOVA was performed with Target Duration (800, 1000, and 1200 ms), Stimulus Size (small and large stimuli), and Stimulus Attribute Relevance (salience and control condition) as three repeated-measurement factors, and Manipulation Interval (manipulation of stimulus size either within the target interval or within the reproduction interval) as a between-subjects factor. To protect against violations of sphericity, Greenhouse-Geisser corrected
p values are reported where appropriate (cf. Geisser & Greenhouse,
1958).
Analysis of variance revealed statistically significant main effects of Target Duration, F(2, 116) = 310.92, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.843, and Stimulus Size, F(1, 58) = 11.32, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.163. The significant main effect of Target Duration indicated longer MRDs with increasing duration of the target interval; MRDs (± SD) were 939 ± 150 ms for the 800-ms target duration, 1063 ± 157 ms for the 1000-ms target duration, and 1168 ± 164 ms for the 1200-ms target duration. A post hoc Scheffé test showed that MRDs of all three target durations differed significantly from each other (p < 0.001). The significant main effect of Stimulus Size on MRD clearly argued for an effect of nontemporal stimulus magnitude on perceived duration. Large stimuli were reproduced longer than small stimuli; MRDs were 1045 ± 146 ms and 1067 ± 155 ms for small and large stimuli, respectively. There was no main effect of Stimulus Attribute Relevance on MRD, F(1, 58) = 0.47, p = 0.49, ηp2 = 0.008; MRDs for the salience and control condition were 1052 ± 152 ms and 1061 ± 161 ms, respectively. Also no main effect of Manipulation Interval on MRD could be established, F(1, 58) = 1.24, p = 0.27, ηp2 = 0.021; MRDs were 1035 ± 147 ms and 1078 ± 150 ms when nontemporal stimulus size was experimentally varied during presentation of the target and reproduction stimulus, respectively.
Statistically significant two-way interactions were revealed for Target Duration and Stimulus Size, F(2, 58) = 4.11, p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.066, as well as for Target Duration and Manipulation Interval, F(2, 116) = 31.93, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.355. It is particularly important to point out that the interaction between Stimulus Size and Manipulation Interval failed to reach statistical significance, F(1, 58) = 3.27, p = 0.08, ηp2 = 0.053. This finding argues against the general notion that stimulus size affects perceived duration by directly affecting the number of pulses. However, a significant three-way interaction of those three factors combined, F(2, 116) = 6.32, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.098, provided evidence for a mutual interference among Stimulus Size, Target Duration, and Manipulation Interval. No other interactions reached the 5% level of statistical significance.
An additional analysis was performed to assess the significant three-way interaction among Stimulus Size, Target Duration, and Manipulation Interval more thoroughly. For this purpose, two-way ANOVA with Stimulus Size and Target Duration as repeated-measurement factors were conducted separately for experimental manipulation of the nontemporal stimulus size during the target interval and the reproduction interval, respectively.
When nontemporal stimulus size was experimentally varied during presentation of the target duration, significant main effects of Target Duration,
F(2, 58) = 257.21,
p < 0.001,
ηp2 = 0.899, and Stimulus Size,
F(1, 29) = 11.00,
p < 0.01,
ηp2 = 0.275, could be observed. Averaged across all three target durations, MRDs were 1019 ± 142 ms and 1052 ± 157 ms for small and large stimuli, respectively. Also the interaction of both these factors became statistically significant,
F(2, 58) = 8.91,
p < 0.001,
ηp2 = 0.235. Subsequent Scheffé post hoc analyses revealed a reliable effect of stimulus size on MRD for the 1200-ms target durations (
p < 0.001), but not for the 1000- and 800-ms target durations. As can be seen from
Figure 2 (left panel), with the 1200-ms target duration, the larger stimulus size resulted in significantly longer reproduced durations than the small one; MRDs were 1216 ± 168 ms and 1146 ± 166 ms for large and small stimuli, respectively.
When nontemporal stimulus size was experimentally varied during the reproduction interval, the significant main effect of Target Duration remained statistically significant,
F(2, 58) = 76.03,
p < 0.001,
ηp2 = 0.724. However, neither a significant main effect of Stimulus Size,
F(1, 29) = 1.55,
p = 0.22,
ηp2 = 0.051, nor a significant interaction of Target Duration and Stimulus Size,
F(2, 58) = 0.85,
p = 0.43,
ηp2 = 0.028, was found in this latter condition. In fact, there was no indication of an effect of stimulus size even for the longest target duration (see
Figure 2, right panel).
The overall pattern of results obtained in the present study clearly indicated that an effect of nontemporal stimulus size on reproduced duration can be observed only if stimulus size is varied during the target interval. During the reproduction interval, an increase in nontemporal stimulus size had no effect on reproduced durations. Furthermore, if there was an effect of stimulus size on reproduced duration, this effect was more pronounced for longer than for shorter target durations. Finally, the absence of a statistically significant main effect of stimulus attribute relevance or any statistically significant interactions including stimulus attribute relevance argued against a direct or a possible intervening effect of attention paid to stimulus size on MRD.