The idea that bottom-up stimulus information plays little role in natural scene viewing contrasts sharply with the strong bottom-up salience effects that arise in experiments employing simpler, more impoverished visual displays, even when participants are given a strong top-down goal (e.g., among many others, Godijn & Theeuwes,
2002; Hickey, van Zoest, & Theeuwes,
2010; Hunt, von Muhlenen, & Kingstone,
2007; Irwin, Colcombe, Kramer, & Hahn,
2000; Lamy & Zoaris,
2009; Siebold, van Zoest, & Donk,
2011; Theeuwes,
1994; van Zoest, Donk, & Theeuwes,
2004a,
2004b; Wolfe,
1994; Zehetleitner, Koch, Goschy, & Muller,
2013). Attention can be captured by irrelevant but salient distractors in color, shape, orientation, and luminance (Siebold et al.,
2011; Theeuwes, De Vries, & Godjin,
2003; Turatto & Galfano,
2000; van Zoest et al.,
2004a). For example, when asked to search for a small element inside a singleton with a unique dimension (either a unique shape or unique color, depending on the block), participants' eye movements were captured when another item in the display happened to be unique in the task-irrelevant dimension. Thus, attention—along with the eyes—is readily captured by salient items (Theeuwes,
1992; Theeuwes et al.,
2003). However, even the role of salience and the extent to which it influences goal-driven eye movements in such basic displays have been hotly debated (e.g., Folk & Remington,
1998; Theeuwes,
2004; van Zoest et al.,
2004a). One emerging idea is that bottom-up stimulus-driven and top-down goal-driven processes both influence oculomotor behavior but operate on different time scales (van Zoest et al.,
2004a). For example, in searching among singletons with differing salience, short-latency, first eye movements were consistently directed toward the most salient singleton, with longer latency and subsequent saccades being predominantly goal driven (Siebold et al.,
2011).