Humans regularly traverse many types of terrain that require visually guided modifications of gait. For example, walking is sometimes made difficult due to the presence of unsafe or undesirable footholds (e.g., puddles, patches of mud or ice, potholes) on a flat ground surface. In such situations, successful locomotion requires walkers to use visual information to identify safe target footholds and modulate stride length, width, and timing to land on those footholds with precision and efficiency.
Given the demands for precision, one might assume that the trajectory of the legs and feet are controlled much like the trajectory of the hand is controlled during reaching to a target, that is, by actively and continuously guiding the effector for as much of the movement as possible. The importance of online control during reaching to stationary targets has been demonstrated by showing that aiming is more accurate when visual information is available throughout the movement compared to when the room lights are extinguished upon movement initiation (Elliott & Allard,
1985; Zelaznik, Hawkins, & Kisselburgh,
1983). Performance advantages with visual information are observed for all but the shortest movements (i.e., those lasting less than 100–150 ms; see Carlton,
1992, for a review), for which perceptual motor delays exceed movement time, preventing the use of visual feedback. Such findings have been interpreted as evidence of a rapid, closed-loop, visual feedback–based process for correcting errors in aiming trajectory (Elliott, Binsted, & Heath,
1999).
Studies of walking demonstrate that humans are capable of rapidly using visual information during the swing phase to improve the accuracy of stepping. When an obstacle suddenly appears (Patla, Beuter, & Prentice,
1991; Weerdesteyn, Nienhuis, Hampsink, & Duysens,
2004) or a target suddenly moves (Reynolds & Day,
2005a), the trajectory of the swinging leg is adjusted within about 120 ms. The extremely short latency of such adjustments compared to voluntary stride modifications and simple reaction time responses has been interpreted as evidence of the involvement of subcortical pathways (Reynolds & Day,
2005a; Weerdesteyn et al.,
2004). Even when the foot target remains stationary, stepping is more accurate when visual information is available throughout the step compared to when vision is occluded at step initiation (Reynolds & Day,
2005b). Converging evidence for the use of visual information during the swing phase has been sought by studying gaze behavior during walking. For example, Hollands, Marple-Horvat, Henkes, and Rowan (
1995) found that when humans walk over a series of irregularly spaced targets, they saccade to the upcoming target shortly before the step to that target is initiated and maintain fixation on the target until shortly before the foot lands on the target. Taken together, these findings provide support for the hypothesis that the accuracy with which humans step to targets is at least partly attributed to the ability to rapidly use visual information to modulate swing leg trajectory while the foot is moving toward the target.
However, when humans walk over extended stretches of complex terrain, there are at least two reasons why precision in the placement of the feet may be achieved in an entirely different manner. First, walkers need to be concerned not only with the upcoming step but also with the terrain further ahead. We recently demonstrated the importance of being able to see the terrain beyond the next step by showing that stepping accuracy degrades and walking speed decreases when the terrain does not become visible until it lies within a single step length (Matthis & Fajen,
2013,
2014). This finding raises questions about the relevance of some of the aforementioned studies for our understanding of visually guided walking over extended stretches of complex terrain. The task in those studies required subjects to take a single step to a target and then stop (Reynolds & Day,
2005a,
2005b) or step over a single obstacle and continue walking (Weerdesteyn et al.,
2004). Although walkers may be able to make rapid adjustments to leg trajectory during a single step, it remains unclear whether visual information is used during the swing phase when walking over a series of irregularly spaced targets when the upcoming terrain must also be taken into account.
Second, during walking over extended stretches of complex terrain, the goal of landing on small targets must be satisfied while simultaneously walking in an energetically efficient manner, which is achieved by exploiting the passive physical forces acting on the body during locomotion. During the single support phase, the human body is mechanically similar to an inverted pendulum (Cavagna & Margaria,
1966) with a large center of mass (COM) supported over a point of rotation at the ankle joint. As the COM travels along an arc defined by the stance leg, the exchange between kinetic and potential energy is very efficient such that the total mechanical energy of the COM remains fairly stable over the course of a step. That is, the inverted pendulum–like structure of the human body makes it possible for energy to be largely conserved during the single support phase, which is central to the energetic efficiency of human walking (Kuo,
2007; Kuo & Donelan,
2010).
When walking over complex terrain, it may not be possible to allow the feet to land where the passive pendulum-like motion takes them. Nonetheless, energetic efficiency is just as important when walking over complex terrain. Therefore, the ability to walk efficiently in such environments is likely to be based on the same principles that allow walkers to be efficient in simpler environments. To take advantage of the body's inverted pendulum–like structure during walking over complex terrain, walkers could use visual information about the upcoming terrain to properly initialize each step so that the body can follow its natural trajectory to a safe foothold (Matthis & Fajen,
2013,
2014). The two determinants of the passive trajectory of the COM during a step are the position of the planted foot and the initial velocity of the COM. As such, to land on a target while exploiting one's inverted pendulum–like structure, a walker could properly position the foot on the preceding step and push off with the trailing leg to redirect the COM so that the passive motion takes the swinging leg toward the target. Indeed, the trajectory of the COM is similar to that of an inverted pendulum even during walking over complex terrain, provided that the walker can identify safe footholds at least two steps in advance, which is the minimum look-ahead distance needed to properly initialize each step (Matthis & Fajen,
2013).
To summarize, during walking over complex terrain, walkers can best exploit the inverted pendulum–like structure of their bodies by placing their feet and tailoring the push-off force during double support of the preceding step in such a way that energetically costly midflight corrections are not needed. In other words, it is to one's advantage to make adjustments before the step is initiated and let the passive forces guide the foot to the target. In this regard, the visual control of stepping on a target may be quite different than the visual control of reaching to a target. Whereas the energetic consequences of visually guided adjustments to the trajectory of the hand during reaching are relatively negligible, executing midflight adjustments to the trajectory of the foot during walking means fighting against more significant forces. Thus, although walkers are capable of using visual information to rapidly adjust the trajectory of the leg during the swing phase, a strategy that allows them to work with rather than against the passive forces that are generated during locomotion would be more energetically efficient.
The aim of the present study was to determine when visual information about a target foothold is no longer necessary to guide foot placement. Subjects walked across a path of irregularly spaced target footholds while attempting to step with a high degree of accuracy at each step. They performed this task in a full vision condition in which the targets were visible for the entire trial and in several limited visibility conditions in which each target became invisible at some point prior to foot placement on that target. If stepping onto a target with the foot is like reaching to a target with the hand (as suggested by Reynolds & Day,
2005b), then visual information should be used during the step to maximize stepping accuracy. On the other hand, if walkers attempt to exploit their inverted pendulum–like structure and follow a ballistic trajectory to the target, then it would be expected that visual information about the target is primarily used before the step is initiated. That is, visual information should no longer play a role once the leg is in the swing phase.