The raw material from which the visual system is required to form an interpretation of the environment is an array of local luminous intensity measurements, converted within the retina to contrast in luminance and chromaticity. Color is not perceived under low levels of ambient luminance, yet the visual system is still able to make sense of the visual scene. It is evident, then, that spatial structure in the luminance contrast measurements is critical to that interpretation. At the level of the primary visual cortex, individual neurons are selective for orientation (Hubel & Wiesel,
1959,
1968), performing the role of oriented spatial filters for contrast in luminance or texture (a second-order property that might be described as a local contrast in luminance contrast (Badcock,
1988; Badcock & Derrington,
1989; Julesz,
1962). The response of adjacent and approximately collinearly arranged filters is enhanced by excitatory lateral connections in the event that both are simultaneously subject to appropriate stimuli (Field & Hayes,
2004; Field, Hayes, & Hess,
1993; Li & Gilbert,
2002). Under such circumstances, it is likely that the stimuli correspond to segments of an extended feature of the projected image of the environment. In this way, the salience of continuous paths is enhanced. The salience of closed paths is further enhanced (Kovacs & Julesz,
1993), suggesting that the visual field is parsed at the boundaries of objects, which are often characterized by a contrast in luminance or texture across the boundary. Identification of objects, however, requires analysis of their shapes (Tan, Dickinson, & Badcock,
2013). A stimulus that has gained some prominence in the psychophysical literature pertaining to the study of shape is the radial frequency (RF) pattern (Almeida, Dickinson, Maybery, Badcock, & Badcock,
2010a,
2010b,
2013,
2014; Bell & Badcock,
2008; Bell, Badcock, Wilson, & Wilkinson,
2007; Dickinson, Almeida, Bell, & Badcock,
2010; Dickinson, Han, Bell, & Badcock,
2010; Dickinson, Harman, Tan, Almeida, & Badcock,
2012; Dickinson, McGinty, Webster, & Badcock,
2012; Dickinson, Mighall, Almeida, Bell, & Badcock,
2012; Loffler,
2008; Loffler, Wilson, & Wilkinson,
2003; Wilkinson, Wilson, & Habak,
1998). Such patterns are contours subtly deformed from circular by a sinusoidal modulation of radius. The number of cycles of modulation of a specific frequency of modulation is varied, and patterns with fewer than the requisite number of cycles of modulation to fill 2
π radians are completed with a circular arc. As cycles of modulation of a particular frequency are added to RF patterns, the amplitude of modulation required for observers to be able to detect deformation of a pattern decreases at a rate faster than predicted by the increasing probability of detecting single cycles of modulation (probability summation). This has been interpreted as indicative of integration of shape information around the patterns (Loffler et al.,
2003). RF patterns, then, provide stimuli that have demonstrably global shape representations and precisely specified shapes. By manipulating the amplitude and frequency of modulation and the number of cycles of that modulation, the relative importance of local properties such as the deviation of the local orientation of the contour from a circle and curvature of the pattern can be examined. Moreover, the widely used choice of the sinusoid as the modulating function is perhaps only a convenience, and the RF pattern, therefore, provides an ideal baseline for the exploration of other modulating functions in the investigation of shape processing in the visual system.