The average critical spacing for monkeys in this study was 0.26
ϕ, where
ϕ denotes target eccentricity. For humans tested under identical conditions, the average critical spacing was 0.25
ϕ. These values are lower than the value of 0.5
ϕ reported in the classic study by Bouma (
1970). Many other reports on crowding also describe values less than 0.5
ϕ. Pelli et al. note, with regard to results from a particular series of experiments, “Bouma was right to say ‘roughly' 0.5. For some of our data, this value drops as low as 0.3” (
2004, p. 1144). Similarly, Chung, Levi, and Legge (
2001) list, in their table 1, prior studies yielding critical spacings as low as 0.1
ϕ and as high as 0.5
ϕ. Variability in measurements of critical spacing can arise from many sources. These include the arrangement of the elements in the display (Toet & Levi,
1992), the degree of similarity between the targets and distractors (Kooi et al.,
1994), the duration of the display (Tripathy & Cavanagh,
2002), the predictability of the display's location (Yeshurun & Rashal,
2010), and the amount of prior training of the observers (Chung,
2007). The outcome is also dependent on the method for computing critical spacing. At present, no single method can be taken as representing a gold standard. The clipped line fit (Pelli et al.,
2004) gives comparatively large readings because it yields a critical distance that lies close to the shoulder of the performance-versus-distance function. The approach of fitting a continuous curve to the data and noting the point at which it intersects a criterial performance level (Tripathy & Cavanagh,
2002) yields comparatively small readings because, with commonly used criteria, the intersection occurs on the slope rather than at the shoulder of the performance-versus-distance function. Our approach falls into the latter category.