Methods of psychophysics offer a way to examine what information is remapped across a saccade onto a spatiotopic reference frame through investigating the transference of aftereffects following perceptual adaptation. There is some evidence from such methods that spatially detailed stimuli are encoded in spatiotopic coordinates, and the degree of this encoding correlates with stimulus complexity, suggesting an increasingly spatiotopic representation along the processing hierarchy of the visual system (Melcher,
2005; Melcher & Colby,
2008), which is supported by some neuroscientific studies (Merriam, Genovese, & Colby,
2007; Nakamura & Colby,
2002; although see Golomb & Kanwisher,
2012). Determining whether specific adaptation-induced aftereffects are retinotopic or spatiotopic, therefore, offers a way to explore how specific visual attributes are encoded with relation to the processing hierarchy of the visual system. The motion aftereffect, for instance, is known to be fixed in retinotopic coordinates (Knapen, Rolfs, & Cavanagh,
2009; Turi & Burr,
2012), reflecting its effect in early visual areas (possibly V1, the first cortical visual area), whereas the positional motion aftereffect (the apparent change in spatial position following motion adaptation) is spatiotopic (Turi & Burr,
2012). It was predicted in the present study that although aftereffects to mean orientation (i.e., the tilt aftereffect) are retinotopic, adaptation to orientation variance may reveal spatiotopic encoding. Although local orientation adaptation results in a retinotopic-specific tilt aftereffect (due to its operating in V1; Knapen, Rolfs, Wexler, & Cavanagh,
2010), the encoding of orientation variance is likely to require the large receptive field properties of later neurons, which are more likely to encode spatiotopically (Melcher,
2005; Melcher & Colby,
2008).