The second primary role of the visual system is to execute movements. Notably, although a growing body of evidence supports the contention that visual
perceptions are mediated by a SSR, a paucity of work has examined whether
actions are similarly influenced. One framework to address this issue is to investigate whether a target property (e.g., eccentricity) representing the SSR of a stimulus-set facilitates the latency and/or amplitude of goal-directed saccades. In one examination of this issue, work by our group (Gillen & Heath,
2014b; see also Gillen & Heath,
2014a) had participants complete prosaccades (i.e., saccade to veridical target location) and antisaccades (i.e., saccade to the target's mirror symmetrical location) to briefly presented targets (i.e., 50 ms) located 10.5° (“proximal” target), 15.5° (“middle” target), and 20.5° (“distal” target) left and right of a common fixation. Importantly, pro- and antisaccades were completed across conditions that differed with regard to the weighting of target eccentricity frequency. In the
control-weighting condition, target eccentricities were presented with equal frequency whereas in the
proximal- and
distal-weighting conditions the “proximal” and “distal” targets were, respectively, presented five times as often as the other eccentricities. It was reasoned that if a SSR influences oculomotor control then the proximal- and distal-weighting conditions should produce amplitudes that are biased in the direction of the most frequently presented target (i.e., the target that represents the “average” eccentricity of the stimulus-set). Results showed that prosaccades were refractory to the different target-weighting conditions. This was an expected finding and one attributed to their mediation via absolute (i.e., metrically precise) sensorimotor transformations specified via retinotopically organized motor maps in the superior colliculus (SC) (Wurtz & Albano,
1980). In other words, the absolute visual information supporting prosaccades is incompatible with the development of a SSR. More notably, antisaccades in the proximal-weighting condition undershot target eccentricity more than their control condition counterpart, whereas the converse pattern was true for the distal-weighting condition. Thus, antisaccade amplitudes were biased in the direction of the most frequently presented target—a result that is entirely compatible with the representation of a stimulus-set via a SSR. In accounting for this finding, we note that antisaccades produce longer reaction times (RTs) (Fischer & Weber,
1992; Hallett,
1978) and less accurate and more variable amplitudes than prosaccades (Dafoe, Armstrong, & Munoz,
2007; Evdokimidis, Tsekou, & Smyrnis,
2006; Heath, Weiler, Marriott, & Welsh,
2011). Further, neuroimaging and electrophysiological evidence from humans and nonhuman primates has shown that the aforementioned antisaccade behavioral “costs” are related to the top-down and two-component process of suppressing a stimulus-driven prosaccade (i.e., response suppression) (Connolly, Goodale, Menon, & Munoz,
2002; Curtis & D'Esposito,
2003; Ford, Goltz, Brown, & Everling,
2005), and the
visual remapping of a target's spatial location to mirror-symmetrical space (i.e., vector inversion) (Moon et al.,
2007; Zhang & Barash,
2000; for comprehensive review, see Munoz & Everling,
2004). Thus, our group proposed that the observed SSR for antisaccades evinces that top-down control renders sensorimotor transformations via the same relative visual information as perceptions.
2 Moreover, we note that our conclusion is consistent with work demonstrating that the spatial location of a target on trial N-1 or N-2 influences the endpoint location for a to-be-performed trial (Rastgardani, Lau, Barton, & Abegg,
2012; see also Abegg, Rodriguez, Lee, & Barton,
2010; Cheng, De Grosbois, Smirl, Heath, & Binsted,
2011; DeSimone, Everling, & Heath,
2015).