The human visual system is highly robust, constantly compensating for changes in the magnitude of blur in retinal images, thus maintaining a relatively constant perception of the world despite changes in the environment (Elliott, Georgeson, & Webster,
2011; Webster,
2011; Webster, Georgeson, & Webster,
2002; Webster, Mizokami, Svec, & Elliott,
2006) or in the subject's optics (Artal, Benito, & Tabernero,
2006; Artal et al.,
2004; Elliott et al.,
2011; Mon-Williams, Tresilian, Strang, Kochhar, & Wann,
1998; Poulere, Moschandreas, Kontadakis, Pallikaris, & Plainis,
2013; Webster,
2011; Webster et al.,
2002). Experiments by Webster and colleagues showed that even brief (i.e., within seconds) exposures to altered blur can result in a measurable change in the neural adaptation states of the visual system (Elliott et al.,
2011; Webster,
2011; Webster et al.,
2002). Adaptation is measured by shifts in the perceived best focus (PBF; aftereffects) following change in stimulation. Another study (Sawides, de Gracia, Dorronsoro, Webster, & Marcos,
2011b) showed that the PBF under natural adaptation is highly correlated with the magnitude of optical blur at the retina, introduced by the higher order aberrations of the eye. Furthermore, when the subject's aberrations were corrected with adaptive optics, adapting to images blurred by the subject's own higher order aberrations did not produce aftereffects, while adapting to images blurred by scaled versions of his or her own aberrations or to the aberrations of other subjects did produce aftereffects (Sawides, de Gracia, Dorronsoro, Webster, & Marcos,
2011a; Sawides et al.,
2012).