Spatial crowding decreases object recognition and conscious visual perception in clutter (Levi,
2008; Pelli, Palomares, & Majaj,
2004; Whitney & Levi,
2011). It was widely acknowledged and demonstrated in a wide variety of tasks and stimulus types, and most commonly found in the periphery of normally sighted people or in the fovea of people with strabismic amblyopia (Bonneh, Sagi, & Polat,
2007; Flom, Weymouth, & Kahneman,
1963; Levi, Klein, & Chen,
2008; Whitney & Levi,
2011). Crowding occurs in a wide variety of tasks, including letter recognition (Bouma,
1971; Flom et al.,
1963; Toet & Levi,
1992), Vernier acuity (Levi, Klein, & Aitsebaomo,
1985; Westheimer,
1975), orientation discrimination (Andriessen & Bouma,
1976; Livne & Sagi,
2007,
2010,
2011; Westheimer,
1976), stereoacuity (Butler & Westheimer,
1978), contrast discrimination (Saarela, Sayim, Westheimer, & Herzog,
2009), face recognition (Louie, Bressler, & Whitney,
2007; Martelli, Majaj, & Pelli,
2005), and grouping (Manassi, Sayim, & Herzog,
2012,
2013), but mostly in the periphery (for a review, see Levi,
2008). The same phenomenon was also termed “contour interaction” in classical studies (e.g., Flom et al.,
1963, although the term “crowding effect” was originally introduced in Ehlers,
1936). A reduction in letter acuity, when presented among other letters, is also termed “flanked acuity” (e.g., Song, Levi, & Pelli,
2014). Here we use the term “crowded acuity” and explore how crowded visual acuity (VA) for a fixed spacing is related to acuity, as opposed to many studies that have focused on the experimental question of the critical spacing dependency on acuity (Chung, Levi, & Legge,
2001; Danilova & Bondarko,
2007; Hariharan, Levi, & Klein,
2005; Pelli et al.,
2004; Tripathy & Cavanagh,
2002).