Significant transfer has also been observed in a double training procedure, wherein individuals are trained on one feature dimension (such as stimulus contrast) in one location, a second feature dimension (such as stimulus orientation) in a second location, and then tested on the initial feature dimension in the second location. Given this design, behavior typically reflects full transfer of the learning that occurred in the initial feature training to the new location (Wang, Zhang, Klein, Levi, & Yu,
2012,
2014; Xiao et al.,
2008; Zhang, Cong, Song, & Yu,
2013; Zhang, Xiao, Klein, Levi, & Yu,
2010). Other work in this domain has outlined, as just a few examples, the role of training task difficulty (Garcia, Kuai, & Kourtzi,
2013; Liu,
1999), stimulus complexity (McGovern, Webb, & Peirce,
2012), training time (Jeter, Dosher, Liu, & Lu,
2010), and characteristics of the transfer tasks (Jeter, Dosher, Petrov, & Lu,
2009) in determining the specificity or generality of learning. In addition to the clear theoretical importance of these results, techniques to induce learning transfer have obvious real-world relevance in rehabilitation paradigms for cortically based visual disorders such as amblyopia (Li, Ngo, Nguyen, & Levi,
2011; Polat, Ma-Naim, & Spierer,
2009; Zhang, Cong, Klein, Levi, & Yu,
2014), cortical blindness (Das, Tadin, & Huxlin,
2014), or age-related declines in vision (DeLoss, Watanabe, & Anderson,
2015), and in improving vision in individuals with normal vision whose jobs or activities involve significant visual demands (Deveau, Ozer, & Seitz,
2014; Schlickum, Hedman, Enochsson, Kjellin, & Fellander-Tsai,
2009).