When failing to recognize the central letter at the central location, the observer is more likely to report a flanker letter than a no-show letter.
Figure 4a presents pre- and posttraining inner-flanker (I2C) and outer-flanker (O2C) substitution errors in partial and whole reports in two of the six observers as examples. To analyze the impact of partial-report training on flanker substitution, we obtained normalized I2C and O2C errors for each observer: We first averaged I2C and O2C error rates, respectively, at stimulus sizes that resulted in 0.2 to 0.8 partial-report rates for each observer. The average I2C and O2C rates were then divided by the total error rates (1 − C2C) and then by the chance level (1/9). The group means of I2C and O2C error rates are shown in
Figure 4b, and their normalized values are shown in
Figure 4c.
A repeated-measures ANOVA with Training (pre- vs. posttest), Error (I2C vs. O2C), and Report (partial vs. whole report) as within-subject factors showed no significant main effects of Training, F(1, 5) = 0.24, p = 0.65, or Error, F(1, 5) = 4.64, p = 0.08. However, there was a significant main effect of Report, F(1, 5) = 17.33, p = 0.009, due to more flanker substitution errors in partial report, which as pair-wise comparisons indicated was contributed by the difference of O2C errors, F(1, 5) = 19.24, p = 0.007, but not the difference of I2C errors, F(1, 5) = 0.29, p = 0.61. There were no significant interactions between Training and Report, F(1, 5) = 1.00, p = 0.36, or Training and Error, F(1, 5) = 0.001, p = 0.98. These results suggest that although partial-report training reduced crowding, it had no significant impact on flanker substitution errors. In addition, more flanker substitution errors, mainly O2C errors, were associated with partial report than with whole report.