September 2015
Volume 15, Issue 12
Free
Vision Sciences Society Annual Meeting Abstract  |   September 2015
The canonical upright in the representation of object orientation
Author Affiliations
  • Miles Hatfield
    Johns Hopkins University, Department of Cognitive Science
  • Emma Gregory
    Johns Hopkins University, Department of Cognitive Science
  • Michael McCloskey
    Johns Hopkins University, Department of Cognitive Science
Journal of Vision September 2015, Vol.15, 238. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/15.12.238
  • Views
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Miles Hatfield, Emma Gregory, Michael McCloskey; The canonical upright in the representation of object orientation. Journal of Vision 2015;15(12):238. https://doi.org/10.1167/15.12.238.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

Some objects are mono-oriented, possessing a canonical or “preferred upright” orientation (Palmer at al., 1981; Jolicoeur, 1985). The implications of canonical orientations for theories of object recognition have been widely discussed (Rock, 1974; Tarr & Pinker, 1989; Ghose & Liu, 2013), but it remains unclear how “canonical” orientations fit into a theory of object orientation representation. How does the orientation representation for an object differ in canonical versus non-canonical orientations? Eight “horizontal” and 8 “vertical” objects (Fig. 1) were each presented in 16 different orientations (canonical and non-canonical) (Fig. 2). On each trial, participants (under working memory load) viewed an object in a “target” orientation and subsequently attempted to select the target from an array of that object in 16 different orientations. In previous research with poly-oriented (no preferred orientation) objects (Gregory and McCloskey, 2010), participants often selected the object primary-axis (OPA) reflection of the target: an “OPA error” (Fig 3a). We predicted that if the “uprightness” of a mono-oriented object is specifically encoded, OPA errors should be affected by canonical orientation: for canonically-oriented vertical targets, an OPA error maintains “uprightness” (Fig. 3b); for a canonically-oriented horizontal target, an OPA error would result in an “upside-down” orientation (Fig. 3c). Indeed, OPA errors were modulated by an Object type (vertical vs. horizontal) X Orientation (canonical vs. not) interaction (F(1,13) = 16.70, p< .05): for vertical objects, OPA errors were equally common for canonical and non-canonical targets (t(13) = .16, n.s.); for horizontal objects, they were significantly reduced for canonical targets (t(13) = 4.01, p< .05) (Fig. 4). Participants apparently represent the “uprightness” of a stimulus, thereby avoiding otherwise common errors that contradict this representation. These results suggest that the canonical orientation of an object affects not just the cognitive processes underlying object recognition but also those involved in representing spatial information.

Meeting abstract presented at VSS 2015

×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×