A neural representation of feature conjunctions in early visual cortex does not, of course, exclude contributions from higher cortical areas. For example, during the feature-binding process, peripheral objects may become linked to central objects that have common visual features in order to disambiguate the peripheral visual information (Barlow,
1981; Wu et al.,
2004). This ambiguity results from the sparse neural representation in the periphery compared to the central visual field. As higher-level areas of the brain are posited to resolve ambiguous information (Leopold & Logothetis,
1996; Logothetis & Schall,
1989; Pack, Berezovskii, & Born,
2001; Shipp, Adams, Moutoussis, & Zeki,
2009; Sun,
2011; M. Wang, Arteaga, & He,
2013), an ambiguity-resolving process mediating feature binding may depend on neural mechanisms in these areas. Previous studies used visual stimuli with conflicting visual information (e.g., binocularly rivalrous stimuli causing bistable perception) to investigate cortical activity associated with resolving ambiguity. For example, monkey V4 neurons show patterns of activity corresponding to perceptual dominance during binocular rivalry established with orthogonally oriented gratings (Leopold & Logothetis,
1996). Also, neural activity in cortical area MT reflects the motion percept during motion rivalry, and correlates with the unambiguous motion percept derived from ambiguous motion features of a moving grid viewed through an aperture (Logothetis & Schall,
1989; Pack et al.,
2001). Additionally, neurons in monkey inferotemporal cortex (IT) respond during the interpretation of ambiguous (morphed) images, and these responses correlate with how these images are interpreted (Liu & Jagadeesh,
2008). Thus, if an ambiguity-resolving process explains the feature-binding errors here, then these higher-level cortical areas involved in ambiguity resolution may contribute also to feature binding.