Introducing a functioning third pigment into the cones of a dichromatic retina must influence the organism's vision because cones with a different functional pigment will respond differently to the light falling on them. The critical question is whether the altered spectral sensitivity will enable the organism to make distinctions based on color (i.e., chromatic distinctions) that it previously could not.
A study by Mancuso et al. (
2009) with squirrel monkeys (
Saimiri sciureus) has led to far-reaching claims to success in restoring normal color vision through gene therapy (Bennett,
2009; Conway et al.,
2010; Liu, Tuo, & Chan,
2011; Mancuso, Mauck, Kuchenbecker, Neitz, & Neitz,
2010; Shapley,
2009).
1 The critical issue is the suggestion that the monkeys could make new higher-dimensional (i.e., trichromatic rather than dichromatic) color distinctions when provided with a third kind of cone sensitivity. To evaluate whether this can really be concluded from the existing evidence, we examine whether the behavioral test that was used required such a higher-dimensional color percept (Jameson et al.,
2001; Jordan, Deeb, Bosten, & Mollon,
2010; Zaidi, Marshall, Thoen, & Conway,
2014). It is evident that the monkeys that received a new cone pigment could subsequently make certain distinctions that they could not make before the treatment. However, is this just because they see the world differently with the modified cones—just as we see the world differently if we place a colorful filter in front of our eyes—or could the monkeys really distinguish between more colors (Makous,
2007)?
Before treatment, the dichromatic monkeys were able to distinguish between colors by comparing the stimulation of middle (M) and short (S) wavelength sensitive cones. Consequently, they could detect targets on a gray background by their color if the ratio of M and S cone stimulation was different for the target than for the background (resulting in the detection thresholds shown by the green curve in
Figure 1; see also figures 2b and c in Mancuso et al.,
2009). Mancuso et al. (
2009) introduced a long (L) wavelength sensitive pigment into some of the monkeys' M cones. If the pigment had been modified in all M cones without any change in the way in which the signals from those cones are interpreted, the treatment would have simply shifted the colors that cannot be distinguished from gray from being ones that maintain the M-to-S cone ratio to being ones that maintain the L-to-S cone ratio (red curve in
Figure 1; this is also explained in Mancuso et al.,
2009, and in Shapley,
2009).
Because the pigment was only changed in a fraction of the cones, the monkey might be able to detect targets that are invisible to the unchanged cones with the modified cones and vice versa. The monkeys might therefore be able to detect a colored target when
either the ratio of L and S cone stimulation
or the ratio of M and S cone stimulation is different from that of the gray background (simulated thresholds indicated by the dashed blue curve in
Figure 1A). To illustrate that this alone could account for how performance changed in the task that was used to evaluate the gene therapy's influence on the monkeys' color vision, in the absence of any further changes in neuronal connectivity, we simulated the possible appearance of several targets to eyes with various combinations of cones. Simulation details are provided in the
Methods section at the end of this paper.