When performing reaching movements to a remembered visual target, the brain uses two classes of reference frames, the ego- and the allocentric reference frame (Battaglia-Mayer, Caminiti, Lacquianiti, & Zago,
2003; Colby,
1998). While a vast number of studies have shown that egocentric reference frames are used to plan and execute goal-directed reaching movements (e.g., Cohen & Anderson,
2002; Lacquaniti & Caminiti,
1998; Thompson & Henriques,
2011), an increasing number of studies also demonstrated an additional influence of allocentric information on reaching behavior, arguing for the integration of multiple reference frames (e.g., Byrne & Crawford,
2010; Diedrichsen, Werner, Schmidt, & Trommershäuser,
2004; Krigolson, Clark, Heath, & Binsted,
2007; Krigolson & Heath,
2004; Obhi & Goodale,
2005; Schütz, Henriques, & Fiehler,
2013,
2015), even in conditions when allocentric cues are covert and fall along a virtual line (Carrozzo, Stratta, McIntyre, & Lacquaniti,
2002).
Byrne and Crawford (2010) further examined how egocentric and allocentric information are combined for reaching. They asked participants to reach for a remembered, visual target (light dot), which was either presented in isolation or was surrounded by four landmarks. After this display disappeared, only the landmarks reappeared but were shifted in space. Even though participants were instructed to ignore the landmarks (using solely egocentric information), averaged reaching endpoints deviated systematically in the direction of the landmark shift. This result shows that allocentric information was integrated into the reach plan and influenced reaching endpoints.