We did not measure perceptual suppression per se in this study, and thus we make no attempt to discuss suppression (i.e., visual awareness) and specifically assign it to particular cortical regions. Furthermore, we acknowledge that just measuring the average BOLD signal (e.g., from V1) during extended periods of rivalry does not allow us to disentangle the separate roles of excitation versus inhibition (subtractive or divisive) in the complex circuit that underlies rivalry (e.g., Said & Heeger,
2013). We can only assume that the BOLD signal provides one global index that reflects a balance between excitation and inhibition (Boorman et al.,
2010,
2015; Brocke, Schmidt, Irlbacher, Cichy, & Brandt,
2008; Muthukumaraswamy, Edden, Jones, Swettenham, & Singh,
2009; Muthukumaraswamy, Evans, Edden, Wise, & Singh,
2012; Schäfer et al.,
2012; Shmuel, Augath, Oeltermann, & Logothetis,
2006). We believe that this index is informative but far from exhaustive (e.g., Fesi & Mendola,
2015). An illustration that it is possible to derive estimates of suppression using functional imaging comes from the study of a related phenomenon called continuous flash suppression, in which one eye is shown a static stimulus while the other eye is shown a dynamically changing stimulus. Thus, the interocular inhibition is not equivalent because of the rapid transients presented only to one eye. This greatly increases the suppression of the other eye's image to the point that rivalrous alternations occur rarely, if at all, and perceptual suppression is even greater than in binocular rivalry (Tsuchiya & Koch,
2005; Tsuchiya, Koch, Gilroy, & Blake,
2006). Several fMRI studies of continuous flash suppression have convincingly demonstrated a correlation between perceptual suppression and reduced BOLD activation in early visual areas (Watanabe et al.,
2011; Yuval-Greenberg & Heeger,
2013) as well as the temporal and parietal cortex (Fang & He,
2005; Hesselmann, Hebart, & Malach,
2011; Jiang & He,
2006).