Visual spatial attention prioritizes information at the selected location improving its sensitivity (Carrasco, Penpeci-Talgar, & Eckstein,
2000; Matsubara, Shioiri, & Yaguchi,
2007; Shioiri, Yamamoto, Kageyama, & Yaguchi,
2002), recognition accuracy (Eriksen & St James,
1986; LaBerge,
1983; Mackeben & Nakayama,
1993; Yeshurun & Carrasco,
1999) and shortening processing time (Carrasco & McElree,
2001; Hikosaka, Miyauchi, & Shimojo,
1993; Posner, Snyder, & Davidson,
1980) relative to stimuli presented at locations to which attention has not been initially allocated. We might expect that attention to a moving stimulus would help detect whether the stimulus is flickering or not. But attention can be focused on a target in at least two different ways: voluntarily (endogenous) attention (Posner et al.,
1980) by directing attention to the target's location, and following it if it moves; or involuntarily (exogenous) attention (Jonides,
1981; Theeuwes,
1991; Yantis & Jonides,
1984) where an abrupt, salient feature draws attention to a target. One property that is quite efficient at drawing attention to a stimulus is motion or flicker. A target that is flickering among other nonflickering stimuli can be identified rapidly without having to search for it (Royden, Wolfe, & Klempen,
2001; Verghese & Pelli,
1992). Although attention may be drawn to transient stimuli, it may not always facilitate the processing of temporal properties of that stimulus (Bocanegra & Zeelenberg,
2011; Bush & Vecera,
2014; Yeshurun & Hein,
2011; Yeshurun & Levy,
2003).