It should be pointed out that the difficulty in identifying the completion process does not only exist for the thin/fat behavioral paradigm. A basic logic of neuroimaging studies to identify the neural mechanism underlying the completion process in Kanizsa-type illusory contour perception is to distinguish neural response to the illusory contour parts from that to the physical contour parts (Treisman,
1999), and such separate neural responses in V1 have been observed in studies using functional magnetic resonance imaging retinotopic methods (Maertens,
2008; Wu et al.,
2012). However, the separate V1 activations to the illusory and physical contour parts could not prove that the activation to the illusory contour parts does not reflect the processing of the physical contour parts, given the close relationship between the processing of the illusory and physical contour parts. Accordingly, shortly after the invention of the thin/fat paradigm, an image classification method demonstrated spatially separate processing of illusory and physical contour parts (Gold et al.,
2000). These results reveal the fact that the processing of the illusory and physical contour parts both contribute to the perception of illusory contours, whereas it remains unclear whether the processing of the illusory contour parts indeed reflects the completion process (Anderson,
2007). Therefore, the current finding of the independence of the effect of the completion process from the effect of the noncompletion process also provides a key support for the identification of the neural mechanism of the completion process. Moreover, the basic design of the comparison between an illusory contour condition and a control condition (rotating contour fragments so that illusory contours disappear) has been widely used in the research of illusory contour perception, including (but not limited to) studies investigating attentional effects (Conci, Müller, & Elliott,
2007a,
2007b; Davis & Driver,
1994; Gurnsey, Poirier, & Gascon,
1996; Li, Cave, & Wolfe,
2008; Wu, Zhou, Qian, Gan, & Zhang,
2015), awareness modulations (Lau & Cheung,
2012; Wang, Weng, & He,
2012), the effect of inducer contrast (Lesher & Mingolla,
1993; Maertens & Shapley,
2008), feed-forward and feed-back processing (Ffytche & Zeki,
1996; Murray et al.,
2002; Stanley & Rubin,
2003), and separate processes at different processing stages (Barlasov-Ioffe & Hochstein,
2008; Foxe, Murray, & Javitt,
2005; Murray, Foxe, Javitt, & Foxe,
2004; Murray et al.,
2006; also see Seghier & Vuilleumier,
2006 for a review of neuroimaging studies). Although such a design is supposed to control for the effect of the processing of the contour fragments, it remains unclear whether the comparison indeed reveals the effect of the interpolation processing when perceiving illusory contours (Seghier & Vuilleumier,
2006). Therefore, the independence of the completion effect from the noncompletion effect as demonstrated in the present study provides a support for the identification of the completion process in a broad research background.