In this study, we focused on low-spatiotemporal-frequency chromatic modulations because this class of stimuli is detected efficiently by humans (Chaparro, Stromeyer, Huang, Kronauer, & Eskew,
1993; Kelly,
1974; Mullen,
1985; Stromeyer, Cole, & Kronauer,
1985; Thornton & Pugh,
1983) and monkeys (Gagin et al.,
2014; Lindbloom-Brown, Tait, & Horwitz,
2014; Merigan,
1989). The cones are the first step for both luminance and chromatic vision, so the relatively high sensitivity to color (or conversely, the insensitivity to achromatic stimuli) must be due to postreceptoral processing. Differences in the sensitivity of neurons in the midget and parasol ganglion-cell pathways may underlie this difference in psychophysical sensitivity, at least for stimuli that stimulate the S-cones minimally (Crook, Lee, Tigwell, & Valberg,
1987; Lee, Martin, & Valberg,
1989; Lee, Martin, Valberg, & Kremers,
1993). These issues lead to several questions that form the basis of our study. First, how much noise is added to chromatic signals downstream of the cones? Second, does the amount of added noise depend on the degree of S-cone modulation in a chromatic stimulus? Third, does the chromatic sensitivity of individual V1 neurons approach the limits imposed by the cones? To answer these questions, we compared the behavior of monkeys performing a chromatic detection task to the performance of a statistical ideal observer that had access to a parameter-free model of currents in the cone outer segments. Results from this comparison revealed that the monkeys' sensitivity to chromatic modulations approached that of the ideal observer (within a factor of 3) for all low-frequency, isoluminant stimuli tested, suggesting that these patterns are transmitted through the visual system with particularly high fidelity. We also found that on average, V1 neurons were substantially less sensitive than the population of cones that presumably fed them but only slightly less sensitive than the monkeys.