Information processing within our sensory systems is highly flexible. The neural responses to sensory stimulation not only depend on the stimulus itself but also on the actual state of the system that changes every moment and with every bit of information processing. This flexibility is illustrated by the phenomenon of adaptation. Adaptation refers to a sensory system's tendency to adjust neural responsiveness after prolonged exposure to a particular sensory stimulation. This tendency represents a general principle of sensory processing and can be observed across different sensory systems, e.g., in visual adaptation (e.g., review by Kohn,
2007), auditory fatigue (Dix, Hallpike, & Hood,
1949; Westerman & Smith,
1984), and vibro-tactile adaptation (Cohen & Lindley,
1938; O'Mara, Rowe, & Tarvin,
1988). Sensory adaptation has marked effects on how we perceive the world. For instance, the perceived size of a target is upscaled after prolonged presentation of a similar stimulus at notably smaller size and, vice versa, downscaled after prolonged presentation of a similar stimulus at notably larger size. This process is referred to as size adaptation. Similar to other size illusions, size adaptation alters the eccentricity of primary visual cortex (V1) activation, suggesting that this particular type of adaptation originates within V1 (Fang, Boyaci, Kersten, & Murray,
2008; Murray, Boyaci, & Kersten,
2006; Pooresmaeili, Arrighi, Biagi, & Morrone,
2013; Schwarzkopf, Song, & Rees,
2011; Sperandio, Chouinard, & Goodale,
2012). Pooresmaeili et al. (
2013) found a correlation between the illusionary size changes and the activation within V1 and explained the illusion by local processes in V1, presumably triggered by the contours of the adapter. Although these data emphasize an important role of early visual areas in size adaptation, they cannot resolve the question of whether the effects observed in these regions emerge solely from feed-forward processes or may be generated via feedback processes from higher cortical regions (Chouinard & Ivanowich,
2014). In terms of a theoretical distinction, the question arises of whether size adaptation is mainly determined by stimulus properties without any or with only minor influence from endogenous and top-down settings. For instance, attention could change size adaptation along with size perception. In fact, attention has previously been shown to modulate sensory processes, such as contrast sensitivity (Carrasco,
2009; Liu, Abrams, & Carrasco,
2009), spatial frequency (Abrams, Barbot, & Carrasco,
2010; Gobell & Carrasco,
2005), and lightness illusions (Economou,
2011; Tse,
2005), as well as size perception (Anton-Erxleben, Henrich, & Treue,
2007; Gobell & Carrasco,
2005).