As mentioned in
Methods, for the analysis we included data between −
π/2 and
π/2 radians to diminish the effects of erroneous counterphase synchronization. As can be seen in
Figure 3 (right column), the data histogram in the LL condition fell fully into the [−
π/2,
π/2] range, and hence all trials could be used (see white portion of
Figure 3). The same is true for the vast majority of trials in the Dc and Lc conditions at 1 and 2 Hz, which fell in the [−
π/2,
π/2] range of the histograms. Even at 3 Hz, almost two thirds of trials occurred in the [−
π/2,
π/2] range.
Table 1 shows the numbers of trials in all conditions excluded after data filtering.
A repeated-measures ANOVA was performed with three within-subject factors: trial type (Dc, negLc, LL), frequency (1, 2, 3 Hz), and ocularity (binocular, monocular). This analysis yielded a significant main effect of trial type, F(2, 14) = 8.40, p = 0.004, η2 = 0.546, and a marginally significant main effect of frequency, F(2, 14) = 3.49, p = 0.059, η2 = 0.333, as well as a significant interaction between frequency and ocularity, F(2, 14) = 4.69, p = 0.028, η2 = 0.401. Other effects were not significant. Thus, there was an effect of trial type on synchronicity perception, which depended on ocularity.
A follow-up repeated-measures ANOVA showed no differences between Dc and negLc over frequency and ocularity, with the smallest p value of .073 for the Frequency × Ocularity interaction and all other ps ≥ 0.104. Conversely, comparison of Dc and LL trial types yielded only a main effect of trial type, F(1, 7) = 17.99, p = 0.004, η2 = 0.720, with all other effects yielding ps > 0.105. Comparison between negLc and LL yielded main effects of trial type, F(1, 7) = 18.85, p = 0.003, η2 = 0.729, and frequency, F(1, 7) = 8.61, p = 0.004, η2 = 0.552, as well as interactions between trial type and frequency, F(2, 14) = 3.79, p = 0.048, η2 = 0.352, and frequency and ocularity, F(1, 7) = 5.79, p = 0.015, η2 = 0.453.
As we obtained a Frequency × Ocularity interaction, we compared perceptually adjusted phases for the Lc trial type in binocular and monocular conditions at each frequency separately. Confirming the visual inspection of mean offsets for the Lc in
Figure 4, we obtained a significant difference only at 1 Hz,
t(7) = −3.181,
p = 0.015, with no difference at other frequencies (
p > 0.876).
Thus, our results show the differences in effect sizes between the control (LL) and each of the experimental trial types but not between the two experimental trial types (Lc and Dc). Furthermore, only analysis including the Lc trial type yielded significant differences between the binocular and monocular conditions, and this was due to the Lc trial type at 1 Hz, with the offset in the binocular condition being atypically large compared to the other offsets.
In testing for which trial types had a phase adjustment significantly different from zero (
Table 2;
Figure 4), we found that the phase in the Dc and Lc trial types differed from zero at 1 and 2 Hz for both binocular and monocular conditions, but only Dc differed at 3 Hz, and only for disparity.
To obtain the temporal difference (Δ
t) in milliseconds between the central and lateral bars in subjective phase adjustment, we used the following relation:
Table 3 summarizes the temporal differences for all three conditions. Positive and negative values of Δ
t correspond to positive and negative values of
φ, respectively. The positive temporal difference in the Dc condition means that participants on average set the perceived motion-change time ahead of the perceived luminance-change time to achieve perceptual synchrony, as if the percept of motion change lagged behind the percept of luminance change. By contrast, in the Lc condition the participants on average set the luminance-change time after the motion-change time, as if the percept of luminance change led the percept of motion change. Overall, this means that luminance is processed faster than motion (in both binocular/stereo and monocular conditions).