However, recent advances in experimental techniques that benefit from genetic engineering, such as optogenetics (Deisseroth,
2011; Fenno, Yizhar, & Deisserith,
2011; Lee et al.,
2012; Pinto et al.,
2013) and two-photon imaging, as well as practical considerations (cost, ethical discussions), have encouraged researchers to consider rodents as a complementary model to study visual perception. In fact, many popular experimental paradigms in rodents rely at least in part on visual cues, such as discrimination tasks (Markham, Butt, & Dougher,
1996) and spatial navigation tasks (Sutherland & Dyck,
1984; Whishaw & Mittleman,
1986), suggesting that rodents might be a useful model for some aspects of visual perception after all. A quick look in history shows that already in the late 1920s rodents were used in behavioral experiments to assess principles of visual perception (Fields,
1936; Higginson,
1926; Krechevsky,
1938; Lashley,
1912; Valentine,
1928; see Zoccolan,
2015, for a review). More recently, a renewed interest in rodent visual perception has shown that the rodent visual system is more advanced than previously thought. For example, Meier, Flister, and Reinagel (
2011) showed that rats are sensitive to the collinearity of gratings, suggesting, as some of the older studies did, that the visual system in rats processes feature conjunctions. Other recent studies suggest that rats are capable to recognize and categorize objects irrespective of changes in location, size, lighting, and viewpoint (Alemi-Neissi, Rosselli, & Zoccolan,
2013; Simpson & Gaffan,
1999; Tafazoli, Di Filippo, & Zoccolan,
2012; Vermaercke & Op de Beeck,
2012; Vinken, Vermaercke, & Op de Beeck,
2014; Zoccolan, Oertelt, DiCarlo, & Cox,
2009).