What we see depends not only on what we look at, but also on preexisting biases and expectations. Ordinarily, the operation of such factors may go unnoticed but their influence can be made patently obvious when the visual stimulus is ambiguous or conflicting. Under those conditions, visual perception tends to become unstable: what we perceive fluctuates over time between alternative visual interpretations, even though what we are looking at remains invariant. Textbook examples of visual instability include binocular rivalry (Alais & Blake,
2005), motion-induced blindness (Bonneh, Cooperman, & Sagi,
2001), ambiguous figures (Long & Toppino,
2004), ambiguous apparent two-dimensional motion (Ternus,
1938) and ambiguous three-dimensional rotational motion (Miles,
1931). One of the interesting properties of multistable perception, as this phenomenon is called, is the tendency for fluctuations in perception to transpire unpredictably over time, without any willful intent on the part of the subject (Brascamp, van Ee, Pestman, & van den Berg,
2005). It is as if the brain attempts to infer what one is seeing but fails to arrive at a stable, unequivocal answer (Leopold & Logothetis,
1999; Sterzer, Kleinschmidt, & Rees,
2009).