There was one peculiar finding in our study. In
Experiment 1, the effect of adaptation was larger when the adapter had a negative polarity than when it had a positive polarity, though this polarity asymmetry was reduced in the dichoptic adaptation (
Experiment 3). Such results would follow naturally from observations that Off channels exhibit greater gain than On channels; Off-channel dominance has been documented by a number of psychophysical studies wherein human observers are reportedly more sensitive to Off stimuli than to On stimuli (Bowen et al.,
1989; Kelly,
1979; Short,
1966). Simple reaction time is also shorter for Off stimuli than for On stimuli (Del Viva, Gori, & Burr,
2006; Komban, Alonso, & Zaidi,
2011; Komban et al.,
2014; Kremkow et al.,
2014; Del Viva & Gori,
2008), and Off signals contribute to discriminating between textures whose histograms are equated in mean and in variance (Chubb, Econopouly, & Landy,
1994; Chubb, Landy, & Econopouly,
2004). In a study whose results perhaps best matches ours, Chubb and Nam (
2000) have shown that Off signals dominate in perceived-contrast discrimination tasks in texture patterns. Physiological studies show that Off units substantially outnumber On units (Ahmad, Klug, Herr, Sterling, & Schein,
2003; Dacey & Petersen,
1992; Jiang, Purushothaman, & Casagrande,
2015) and have greater population response in retina, LGN and V1 (Fiorentini, Baumgartner, Magnussen, Schiller, & Thomas,
1990; Yeh, Xing, & Shapley,
2009; Zemon, Gordon, & Welch,
1988). Ratliff, Borghuis, Kao, Sterling, and Balasubramanian (
2010) recently pointed out that such dominance of the Off mechanism is consistent with a processing constraint designed to maximize information availability for natural images. The On/Off asymmetry observed herein also could be accounted for by adaptations to such statistical structure of natural scenes.