If there were shape priming (i.e., congruent prime shape facilitates target shape discrimination,
Figure 1C), then we should expect an influence of Shape congruency in the shape-discrimination task, as either a main or interaction effect. While we found no main effect (see
Supplementary Table S2 for full ANOVA results), interactions involving Shape congruency were significant—namely Category congruency × Shape congruency,
F(1, 26) = 5.62,
p = 0.025,
ηp2 = 0.18, and Target category × Target elongation × Shape congruency,
F(1, 26) = 5.88,
p = 0.023,
ηp2 = 0.18—indicating that prime shape indeed affected RTs in the shape-discrimination task. The three-way interaction was robust to the inclusion of more trials with long RTs when using the “mean ±3
SD” outlier definition,
F(1, 26) = 6.26,
p = 0.019,
ηp2 = 0.19, and we further explored it by testing the Target elongation × Shape congruency interaction separately for tool and animal targets (
Figure 3). For tool targets, the two-way interaction was significant,
F(1, 26) = 4.30,
p = 0.048,
ηp2 = 0.14, because the Shape congruency effect was larger for elongated targets (589 vs. 600 ms) than for nonelongated targets, where it was numerically reversed (647 vs. 635 ms). Post hoc tests revealed that the facilitatory effect was significant,
t(26) = 2.15,
p = 0.041, while the reversed effect was not,
t(26) = −1.42,
p = 0.166. Using the “mean ±3
SD” outlier definition (
Figure 3B), post hoc tests were significant for the facilitatory effect (606 vs. 623 ms),
t(26) = 2.10,
p = 0.046, and the reversed effect (682 vs. 661 ms),
t(26) = −2.14,
p = 0.042. For animal targets, the two-way interaction was not significant,
F(1, 26) = 0.09,
p = 0.769,
ηp2 < 0.01.