On a neuronal level, visual and vestibular signals are combined relatively early in sensory processing pathways (Dichgans, Diener, & Brandt,
1974; Gu, DeAngelis, & Angelaki,
2007), suggesting a close interdependence between visual and vestibular processing. Body tilts away from upright lead to decreased reliability of vestibular sensory estimates (Graybiel & Patterson,
1955; Quix,
1925), causing a reweighting of other sensory inputs, including vision (Dichgans et al.,
1974; Fetsch, Turner, DeAngelis, & Angelaki,
2009). This can lead to modifications in visual perception, for example, in the perception of the orientation of lines or objects (Aubert,
1861; Mikellidou, Cicchini, Thompson, & Burr,
2015). The perception of the own body and its orientation relative to extrapersonal space is created by a combination of vision, body position, and gravity (Dyde, Jenkin, Jenkin, Zacher, & Harris,
2009; Harris, Herpers, Hofhammer, & Jenkin,
2014). In a supine body position, the direction of gravity in relation to the body changes, causing a shift in the perceptual upright that could also affect visual motion perception. Varying the body position can lead to systematic biases in estimating the direction of forward movements relative to the horizon from optic flow (Bourrelly, Vercher, & Bringoux,
2010) and increase the threshold of vestibular as well as visual heading direction discrimination close to straight ahead (MacNeilage, Banks, DeAngelis, & Angelaki,
2010). Similarly, effects of body orientation have been found for distance estimation (Harris & Mander,
2014) and the sensation of vection (i.e., the illusory perception of self-motion that develops during prolonged viewing of optic flow) (Kano,
1991; Thilo, Guerraz, Bronstein, & Gresty,
2002).