The occurrence of microsaccades during fixation has been reported since 1907 (Dodge,
1907). Thereafter, multiple studies using different recording techniques concluded that microsaccades are binocular coordinated movements with similar timing, directions (conjugacy), and velocities in the two eyes (Ditchburn & Ginsborg,
1953; Krauskopf, Cornsweet, & Riggs,
1960; Schulz,
1984; St. Cyr & Fender,
1969; Yarbus,
1967). Zuber, Stark, and Cook (
1965) demonstrated that saccades, microsaccades, and corrective saccades (see next paragraph) lie on a single linear peak velocity versus amplitude relationship, the so-called main sequence, and deduced that these movements result from a common physiological mechanism. Boyce (
1967) observed that the amplitude distribution of microsaccades during monocular fixation exhibits a cut-off around 12′ with a few outliers around 20′. A review of the fixation data from 14 experiments by Ditchburn and Foley-Fisher (
1967) confirmed these results and indicated a median microsaccadic amplitude during fixation of 4′–5′. More recent studies reported a larger mean microsaccade size, between 13.7′ and 38.4′ (see the review by Martinez-Conde, Macknik, Troncoso, & Hubel,
2009). The disagreement about the amplitude characteristics of microsaccades (Collewijn & Kowler,
2008) might be traced in part to different levels of subject expertise (Cherici, Kuang, Poletti, & Rucci,
2012; Martinez-Conde et al.,
2009) and recording conditions, considering that recordings were mainly made with experienced subjects using the optical lever technique in the 1950s and 1960s, whereas recent studies of fixation used video-based eye tracking systems on naïve participants. Cherici et al. (
2012) also emphasize that previous studies typically assumed that the distribution of gaze positions during fixation is normal, an assumption that does not hold. It is also acknowledged that although the distinction between microsaccades and saccades based on their size (<1°) is arbitrary, this distinction is practical in that it captures approximately 90% of the nonvoluntary fixational saccades (Martinez-Conde et al.,
2009).