Yet, manual reaction time shows the same correlation with the Snellen acuity of the amblyopic eye as saccadic latency (Hamasaki & Flynn,
1981), and the motor component of the refractory period of a saccade would not affect the timing of hand movements. However, the refractory period for a saccade involves a shift in attention prior to initiation (Kowler et al.,
1995). Microsaccades, like larger saccades, also involve attentional shifts before initiation (Chen, Ignashchenkova, Their, & Hafred,
2015; Yuval-Greenberg, Merriam, & Heeger,
2014). Divided attention increases reaction time (Ninio & Kahneman,
1974), so perhaps when microsaccades are made immediately before the target appears, visual attention is divided between the attentional shifts accompanying the microsaccades and the attention directed to monitoring target onset, resulting in longer manual reaction times. There is evidence that, in normal observers, faster manual reaction times are associated with a reduction in the frequency of microsaccades (Betta & Turatto,
2006; Kliegl, Rolfs, Laubrock, & Engbert,
2009). We speculate that, for the strabismic amblyope, the numerous microsaccades, made while struggling to maintain fixation, result in all types of reactions being delayed by a fluctuating wave of refractoriness and the accompanying shifts in attention. Because the degree of unsteadiness is correlated with LogMAR acuity (Birch,
2013; Chung et al.,
2015), it follows that manual reaction time and saccadic latency are also correlated with LogMAR acuity.