A multilevel logistic-regression analysis was done for the figure–ground-task responses. A likelihood-ratio test showed that including main effects for geometric cue and location of motion was a significant improvement over an unconditional-means model (i.e., containing only an intercept)—comparing models that include location of motion to the unconditional means model:
LR = 553.3,
df = 7,
p < 0.001; comparing models that include location of motion and geometric cue to the one that only includes location of motion:
LR = 104.2,
df = 5,
p < 0.001. The addition of the color factor (i.e., whether the target region is dark or light) to the model that includes the two main effects of location of motion and geometric cue was also a significant improvement over the model,
LR = 21.18,
df = 6,
p < 0.01. The interaction between location of motion and geometric cue was also found to be a significant addition,
LR = 35.51,
df = 15,
p < 0.01, yielding our final model. As expected, the regions that contained the geometric cues were more likely to be seen as figural when the geometric cue was convexity,
M = 0.52,
SE = 0.05, compared to when it was just symmetry,
M = 0.36,
SE = 0.06. Apart from the obvious fact that in one condition we have two geometric cues (convexity and symmetry) whereas in the other there is only one (symmetry), it is also known that the symmetric displays here result in a rather weak figural bias (Froyen et al.,
2013). Tukey pair-wise comparisons, done between the three different conditions of the location of motion, revealed the following effects. The proportion of times the convex/symmetric regions were seen as figural in the condition where regions on both sides of a boundary had motion (i.e., column 3 in
Figure 3) was significantly higher than the proportion of times the moving regions were seen as figural in the condition where only convex/symmetric regions were moving (i.e., column 1 in
Figure 3),
p < 0.05, and also in the condition where only nonconvex/asymmetric regions were moving (i.e., column 2 in
Figure 3),
p < 0.001. The interaction between geometric cue and location of motion is seen when the effect of location of motion in the convexity condition is compared to its effect in the symmetry condition. In the condition in which nonconvex regions had moving texture, the proportion of times the moving regions were seen as figural was significantly lower than chance level (the rightmost, turquoise bar on the top left graph of
Figure 5); however, when the part-wise convex regions had the moving texture, the proportion increase dramatically to chance level (the red bar on the right side of the top left graph of
Figure 5). When both sides had moving texture, the part-wise convex regions were seen as figural almost all the time (the red bar on the right side of the top right graph of
Figure 5). This strong effect of motion location was not observed in the symmetry condition (i.e., on the left side of the top left graph of
Figure 5, it is seen that the difference between the red and the turquoise bars is relatively small, and it is seen from the red bar on the left side of the top right graph of
Figure 5 that the proportion only goes up to chance level even when both sides had moving texture). Thus, whether accretion-deletion is introduced onto the convex/symmetric region or onto the nonconvex/asymmetric region makes a big difference when the geometric cue is convexity, but little difference when it is symmetry. Further investigation of the main effect of color shows that there is no structured effect of color on figure–ground responses; while some subjects have a bias towards dark regions (
n = 9), others have a bias towards light regions (
n = 4).