The level where certain illusions occur in the visual hierarchy might explain why some illusions affect visually guided action when others do not (Milner & Dyde,
2003). Our adaptation method modulates neural responses in early visual areas (Pooresmaeili et al.,
2013), but still we found a dissociation between very quick and normal saccade responses. We argue that the quick responses rely on signals that are uninformed by adaptation. This supports studies suggesting that the latency of actions determines whether stimuli are coded in an egocentric or an allocentric frame of reference (Gentilucci et al.,
1996; Hu, Eagleson, & Goodale,
1999; Fischer,
2001; Westwood et al.,
2000; Westwood & Goodale,
2003). Other studies have found that saccades with short latencies are deceived by the Müller–Lyer illusion while saccades with long latencies are not (de Grave & Bruno,
2010; van Zoest & Hunt,
2011). However, the Müller–Lyer illusion arises at later stages in the visual hierarchy (Weidner & Fink,
2007; Walter & Dassonville,
2008; Plewan, Weidner, Eickhoff, & Fink,
2012), even involving dorsal stream areas (de Brouwer, Smeets, Gutteling, Toni, & Medendorp,
2015). A recent study has found that presentation duration, not saccade reaction time, determines whether action and perception are deceived by the illusion: Longer presentation durations resulted in smaller effects (de Brouwer, Brenner, Medendorp, & Smeets,
2014). This finding is consistent with results by our lab, where saccadic suppression of displacement, another illusion that deceives perception but not action, decreases as a function of presentation duration (Zimmermann et al.,
2013).