The notion of order appears in Gestalt psychology (Koffka,
1935), and is related to arrangements of objects where their relative placement has significance. As such, order may consist of specific regularities (laws), and these may interact synergistically (Wagemans, Wichmann, & de Beeck,
2005). It was suggested that the visual system seeks order (e.g., symmetry) so as to make sense of sensory signals. The ability of the visual system to detect regularities has been proposed as a method of compressing information to reduce redundancy (Attneave,
1954). Gestalt principles considered under this view are functions of the perceptual machinery that group information together and thus provide an economical description of visual reality. Order in the form of repetitive and symmetrical patterns is also aesthetically preferred (e.g., Newell, Murtagh, & Hutzler,
2013) and has been utilized historically for artistic and architectural purposes. In nature, perfect order rarely exists, and the visual system has to cope with intermediate states of order. Subjective assessments of the degree of order are common in everyday life but also in scientific research when visual patterns are examined (Cohen, Baum, & Miodownik,
2011; Cook,
2004; Marinari et al.,
2012). Partial order is particularly relevant to biology and biomedicine, because living systems tend to be well-, but not perfectly, ordered. For example, in the mammalian eye, the spacing of the parafoveal receptors is less than perfectly regular, preventing Moiré-like aliasing (Wässle & Boycott,
1991). On the other hand, generation of disorder is associated with aging and disease (Guillaud et al.,
2004; Hu, Li, Wang, Gou, & Fu,
2012; Sudbø, Marcelpoil, & Reith,
2000). As a texture attribute, the degree of order is essential for texture discrimination and segmentation (Bonneh, Reisfeld, & Yeshurun,
1994; Ouhnana, Bell, Solomon, & Kingdom,
2013; Vancleef et al.,
2013). Order also interacts with other perceptual dimensions, and its precise control in stimuli configurations is crucial for psychophysical experiments. For example, the degree of positional order of elements is known to affect perceived numerosity (Allik & Tuulmets,
1991; Dakin, Tibber, Greenwood, Kingdom, & Morgan,
2011; Ginsburg & Goldstein,
1987). For contour integration tasks, patterns of intermediate positional order should be carefully generated in ways to avoid density cues (Demeyer & Machilsen,
2012; Machilsen, Wagemans, & Demeyer,
2015).