A few studies have examined the difference in gaze-direction perception between monoscopic and stereoscopic conditions. Imai, Sekiguchi, Inami, Kawakami, and Tachi (
2006) compared judgment errors in live, monoscopic, and stereoscopic conditions for horizontally and vertically averted gazes. Vertically averted gaze was judged most accurately in the live condition, followed by stereoscopic, and least accurately in the monoscopic condition. Presentation condition had no effect on the accuracy of the judgments of horizontally averted gaze. Gamer and Hecht (
2007) compared computer-generated monoscopic and stereoscopic stimuli with live stimuli and concluded that there were no differences between the widths of the cone of gaze, except that at a far viewing distance (5 m), the live-condition cone of gaze was narrower than in the pictorial conditions. The authors speculated that the difference originated from image resolution or differences in the looker attributes. More recently, West (
2015) studied gaze-direction perception in monoscopic and stereoscopic conditions, with results indicating that perceived directions of gaze between monoscopic and stereoscopic conditions do not differ. However, the stimuli used in that experiment were acquired with a stereoscopic camera setup where both cameras were offset from the midline, one to the left and one to the right, and the looker fixated on the point midway between the cameras. This is analogous to a real-life situation where the looker fixates on the bridge of the observer's nose, and consequently, we call this type of stereoscopic direct gaze the
bridge-of-nose gaze. As already mentioned, when looking at a face at a relatively short distance, a looker mainly fixates on the left or the right eye of the observer, not on the bridge of the nose. Thus, when viewed binocularly, each eye of the observer receives a different oculocentric gaze direction: One eye receives a direct gaze and the other eye receives a slightly averted gaze. We name this type of direct gaze
semidirect gaze to distinguish it from the bridge-of-nose gaze. In the visual system, oculocentric directions received by the two eyes transform into a single egocentric direction perceived in the cyclopean view. An observer perceives the cyclopean view to originate from a location between the two eyes, sometimes called the
cyclopean eye or the
egocenter. The egocentric perceived directions of objects follow the Wells–Hering laws (Ono & Mapp,
1995). In particular, Wells–Hering Law 3c states that the perceived cyclopean direction is the mean of the physical oculocentric directions if the retinal images are successfully fused. The law yields a perceived visual direction that is veridical with respect to the physical environment. Similarly, such averaging over the oculocentric gaze directions would yield a veridical perceived cyclopean direction, and appears to hold for the stereoscopic bridge-of-nose gaze (West,
2015). However, little is known about egocentric perceived gaze direction in the semidirect gaze condition.