When thinking about the mechanism responsible for the adaptation, studying the roles of individual vergence components, such as proximal or fusional, requires controlling the activity of the other components, either by fixing their level and creating a cue conflict situation (Ramsdale & Charman,
1988; Smithline,
1974) or by reducing the available information to make a cue uninformative and open loop (Schor,
1979; Wong et al.,
2001). In the current study, the approach taken by Henson and North (
1980) was adapted for use with children. An objective eye-tracking approach was used here to avoid asking young children for subjective responses, and accommodation responses were measured simultaneously so that the relationship between the motor systems could be assessed. Based on the Maddox classification of vergence response components, proximal and accommodative vergence cues remained constant at the 95 cm distance throughout the study. Therefore, these components should not have demonstrated adaptation in the presence and absence of the prism. The disparity cue for fusional vergence was also removed during measurement of heterophoria and, therefore qualitatively, the adaptation demonstrated in the current study is consistent with tonic adaptation described in previous studies of adults (Carter,
1965; Maddox,
1893). Previous studies that have measured tonic vergence positions directly in infants show inconsistent findings (Aslin,
1986; Rethy,
1969). When both the accommodation and vergence loops were open during tonic vergence measurements, environmental and voluntary factors could still induce vergence response changes and therefore complicate the study (Rosenfield,
1997). In recent oculomotor models, slow fusional vergence (Schor,
1992) and adaptive time components (Hung,
1992) were proposed to underlie this adaptation in the sense of Maddox's tonic vergence. In one study of adults, vergence positions were found to adapt to an increased IPD generated using an optical device (Fisher & Ciuffreda,
1990), which also simulates increasing head growth experienced during infancy and childhood. Given that infants and young children are typically hyperopic (Ingram, Arnold, Dally, & Lucas,
1990; Mutti et al.,
2005) and have a narrower IPD than adults (MacLachlan & Howland,
2002; Pryor,
1969), this form of adaptation would be helpful in relieving stress on fast fusional disparity-driven vergence (Judge,
1996; Mitchell & Ellerbrock,
1955; Ogle & Prangen,
1951; Sreenivasan & Bobier,
2015) at an age when refractive strabismus may develop (Parks,
1958).