Figure 6 shows the average estimated position of the light source above the probe (pink disks) for the nine different positions of the light source above the scene (white disks). The error bars on the pink disks show the 95% confidence intervals. We also show the raw data in
Figure 7 as scatter plots of the estimated positions of the light source in each scene, including the 50% prediction ellipses for each light source position. The results show that the pattern of the estimated light positions was indeed mirrored when the scene was mirrored (compare Scene A and Scene B). Scene C, replacing the bowling pin with another pentagon body, resulted in a systematic contraction of the estimated light position along the y-axis (slant angle) near both pentagon bodies. Scene D, replacing both pentagon bodies with bowling pins, finally resulted in estimated light positions close to veridical ones. Furthermore, we found that the variance of the estimated light position along the y-axis was always larger than along the x-axis (as evidenced by the error bars in
Figure 6 and the shape of the ellipses in
Figure 7).
Two 3 (row) × 3 (column) repeated-measures analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were performed: one for the estimated distances along the x-axis and one for the estimated distances along the y-axis (hereafter referred as X-position and Y-position) of the light on the probe, for each of the four scenes separately. We found that in all four scenes, the estimated X-position was significantly influenced by the column, in which the light source on the scene was located: Scene A, F(2, 88) = 420, p < 0.001; Scene B, F(2, 88) = 300, p < 0.001; Scene C, F(2, 88) = 423, p < 0.001; Scene D, F(2, 88) = 629, p < 0.001. Similarly, the estimated Y-position was significantly influenced by the row: Scene A, F(2, 88) = 164, p < 0.001; Scene B, F(2, 88) = 116, p < 0.001; Scene C, F(2, 88) = 69, p < 0.001; Scene D, F(2, 88) = 232, p < 0.001. These results suggest that, generally, the observers were able to distinguish different light directions in all four scenes.
Nevertheless, the estimated light source positions were closer to the veridical ones in Scene D with the two bowling pins than in the other scenes, as shown in
Figure 6 and
Figure 7. Together with the finding that the average estimated light position was often contracted around the position of the pentagon body, we assumed that the globally spherical smoothly curved bowling pins, in comparison with the facetted pentagon shapes, helped the observers to perceive the veridical light direction.
Both Scenes C and D had a pair of the same objects (i.e., pentagon bodies and bowling pins) standing side by side in the scenes. The estimated light position along the x-axis in these two scenes was much closer to the veridical value than in Scenes A and B. According to a post hoc analysis, the absolute difference (AD) between the estimated light position and the veridical value (expressed in terms of distance in X for the position of the light disk on the LCD screen) was significantly smaller for Scene C (M = 1.81, SE = 0.08) and Scene D (M = 1.56, SE = 0.07) than for Scene A (M = 2.58, SE = 0.09) and Scene B (M = 2.90, SE = 0.11). Thus, we conclude that symmetric arrangements within a scene improve the estimation of the tilt direction of the light source.
The results above showed that human perception of the light direction in a real scene was systematically dependent on scene layout and content.