Binocular rivalry is arguably the most frequently used perceptual suppression paradigm to study the relationship between perceptual organization and visual awareness. In binocular rivalry, different images presented to the left and right eye compete for visibility, leading to the perceptual suppression of one of the images (Alais & Blake,
2005; Wheatstone,
1838). A specific variant of this paradigm, called continuous flash suppression (CFS; Tsuchiya & Koch,
2005), in which one of the images flashes at a continuous rate of usually 10 Hz, has become a popular method of studying unconscious visual processing, as it effectuates relatively large control over several characteristics of the suppression process, like its onset and its duration. Although there are some inconsistencies in the literature, there is accumulating support for the idea that many relatively basic, low-level visual processes still take place under CFS and binocular rivalry more generally, whereas there is less consistent evidence that this is also the case for higher level visual processing (for recent reviews, see Gayet, Van Der Stigchel, & Paffen,
2014, and Yang, Brascamp, Kang, & Blake,
2014). For instance, it has recently been shown that low-level effects such as collinear facilitation and simultaneous brightness contrast still occur for perceptually suppressed flankers and surfaces, respectively (Harris, Schwarzkopf, Song, Bahrami, & Rees,
2011; Hayashi & Murakami,
2015). In contrast, for a higher level brightness phenomenon like the Craik-O'Brien-Cornsweet effect, awareness of the central luminance edge appears necessary for the effect to occur (Masuda et al.,
2011). Furthermore, for cast shadows, the low-level properties are still processed under CFS, as indicated by the motion aftereffect that they generate, but their suppression strongly diminishes illusory motion perception in depth (Khuu, Gordon, Balcomb, & Kim,
2014). Finally, for the classical Kanizsa illusory surface completion conflicting evidence has been reported. Whereas Wang, Weng, and He (
2012) showed that Kanizsa pacmen break through suppression faster when they induce an illusory shape compared to when they are oriented randomly, several other studies failed to obtain convincing evidence for the illusory shape to be represented during perceptual suppression (Harris et al.,
2011; Moors, Wagemans, van Ee, & de-Wit,
2015; Sobel & Blake,
2003).