The critical point to notice in the description of their simulation that makes their reported result unexpected is that the effect of the Müller–Lyer illusion is described as being modeled in accordance with the Weber–Fechner law, where the magnitude of the illusion is proportional to the size of the visual stimulus. This relationship is intuitive and has been supported empirically in humans (Tudusciac & Nieder,
2010). However, if this is the case, then the distance between the means of the two perceived length distributions will be different between the different illusory conditions (
Figure 1). If we then consider that all simulated distributions have a constant standard deviation of 1.2 cm, we should expect the results of the simulation to show a change in
d′ between the tails-in and tails-out conditions. Indeed, when we ran this simulation, a two-tailed, paired-samples
t test showed a significant difference in
d′ between these two conditions (
d′ tails-in = 1.43 ± 0.14,
d′ tails-out = 2.02 ± 0.18,
t(9) = 9.499,
p < 0.001;
Figure 2A). This raises the question of where the discrepancy lies between our simulation and that of Witt et al. (
2015). Given that they reported a constant standard deviation for all simulated distributions, it follows from SDT that the only way to maintain a constant value of
d′ while changing the criterion is to shift the two evidence distributions by the same constant. We modified our simulation to do this by shifting the means of the tails-out and tails-in distributions relative to the distributions in the no-tails condition by ±13% of 6 respectively, and the resulting SDT measures matched those reported in Witt et al. (
d′ tails-in = 1.81 ± 0.13,
d′ tails-out = 1.69 ± 0.18,
t(9) = 1.55,
p = 0.16;
Figure 2B).