To investigate the relationship between the trajectory deviation and the detection times further, we studied the deviation on the
x-axis (
x-deviation) prior and posterior to the moment the target was detected. Given that only target location affected the AUC, we focused on the
x-deviation only on this variable. As the alternative responses are spatially distinguished on the
x-axis, an analysis of
x-deviations can index the ongoing competition across time. For example, greater deviations can indicate that the incorrect response is considered a valid alternative. As with velocity, we analyzed the time course of
x-deviation in relation to when the target was detected (detection time) and the end of target verification (end of the verification epoch). We hypothesized that an unusual location should increase the deviation. As accumulating evidence toward an
absent response is aimed at accumulating confidence that the target is not in the scene, the
x-deviation should accordingly begin to increase during the detection epoch as it is during this epoch that the
target-absent response competes with the
target-present response. As we did for velocity profiles, for each trajectory, we centered the time coordinates on the detection time so that a 0-ms time coordinate indicates the moment on the response trajectory when the target was detected. The
x-deviation was the difference between the coordinate on the
x-axis of each point of the response trajectory and the
x-coordinate of its perpendicular projection point on the ideal response trajectory (straight line from
start to
present button). A positive
x-deviation value denotes a deviation toward the
absent response compared to the ideal trajectory. We smoothed the
x-deviation profiles of each trajectory by using a convolution with a triangular kernel over a sliding window of 21 time steps.
Figure 5A displays the
x-deviation as a function of time centered on detection of the target for usual (solid line, dark gray error) and unusual target locations (dashed line, light gray error). The 95% Wald confidence intervals included in
Figure 5A were estimated with intercept-only LMMs for each time step. As stated previously, these LMMs included the intercept's variation across both participants and scenes as random parameters. Two observations are relevant here. First, as with the results with the AUC, the overall deviation was greater for targets in an unusual location than in a usual location. This was the case from 304 ms before target detection to 640 ms after target detection (60 steps). For targets in an unusual location, the
x-deviation attained its maximum value (
b = 0.115, 95% CI [0.087, 0.143]) around 384 ms after target detection, corresponding to the average end of target verification (343 ms, dashed vertical line). For targets in a usual location, the maximum
x-deviation (
b = 0.063, 95% CI [0.045, 0.08]) occurred 560 ms after the target detection, which corresponds to an approximate 200-ms delay after the end of target verification (360 ms, solid vertical line). This result may seem counterintuitive, but given that the target was detected sooner when in a usual location and thus verified sooner, participants had much more time to finish their movement and validate their response before the time granted to respond elapsed. Second, we observed a significant deviation toward the
absent response before target detection. The trajectory deviated up to 128 ms before target detection for targets in a usual location and 512 ms for targets in an unusual location. As reported previously, the deviation was significantly greater for targets in unusual locations starting 304 ms before target detection (
Figure 5A).