Figure 7E and
7F show how the accuracy and the bias of the tilt estimates vary as function of the image cue value for each of the individual cues (colored curves) and for the case in which the three cue values agree (black curve). When the cue values agree, estimation accuracy is considerably better (in agreement with the reduced bias and variance of the estimates shown in
Figure 7C,
D). This fact could be exploited by the visual system because the cue values are available to the observer (see
Discussion) Next, we examine tilt estimates given both the disparity and luminance cue values. The estimates for all combinations of luminance and disparity cue values,
E(
ϕr|
ϕl,
ϕd), are shown in
Figure 8A. The pattern of results is intuitive but complex. Depending on the particular values of the disparity and luminance cues, we see several different types of behavior: disparity dominance, cue averaging, and cue switching. For example, when disparity equals 90°,
E(
ϕr|
ϕl,
ϕd = 90), we observe disparity dominance; that is, the luminance cue exerts almost zero influence on the estimate (vertical midline of
Figure 8A; see
Figure 8B inset). On the other hand, when luminance equals 90°,
E(
ϕr|
ϕl = 90,
ϕd), the disparity cue exerts a strong influence on the estimate (horizontal midline of
Figure 8A). When luminance and disparity agree,
E(
ϕr|
ϕl =
ϕd), the single-cue estimates are approximately averaged (positive oblique of
Figure 8A). When luminance and disparity disagree by 90°,
E(
ϕr||
ϕl −
ϕd| = 90), the best estimates switch from 0° to 90° abruptly when the disparity cue approaches ∼65° and then switches abruptly back from 90° to 0° when the disparity cue approaches ∼115°. All of these effects can be seen more readily by examining the value of the estimate as a function of the disparity cue for different luminance cue values (
Figure 8B) and as a function of the luminance cue for different disparity cue values (
Figure 8C).