There is also a close link between eye and hand movements. Many studies show that eye movements occur naturally when observers engage in reaching, grasping, pointing, or hitting (Diaz, Cooper, Rothkopf, & Hayhoe,
2013; Hayhoe & Ballard,
2005; Hayhoe, McKinney, Chajka, & Pelz,
2012; Land,
2006; Land & McLeod,
2000; Mrotek & Soechting,
2007; Ripoll, Bard, & Paillard,
1986; Soechting & Flanders,
2008). Professional athletes and other task experts show more accurate and less variable eye movements in the field. For instance, expert cricket batsmen make a saccade to the predicted bounce location of a consistently bowled ball; experts' saccades are more accurate and occur earlier than novices' saccades (Land & Furneaux,
1997; Land & McLeod,
2000). Moreover, eye and hand movements are spatially and temporally coordinated. Gaze leads the hand by up to 1 s (Ballard, Hayhoe, Li, & Whitehead,
1992; Land,
2006; Sailer, Flanagan, & Johansson,
2005; Smeets, Hayhoe, & Ballard,
1996) and gaze locations depend on task requirements during object manipulation (Belardinelli, Stepper, & Butz,
2016; Johansson, Westling, Bäckström, & Flanagan,
2001). Gaze is anchored on the target in pointing tasks (Gribble, Everling, Ford, & Mattar,
2002; Neggers & Bekkering,
2000) and when hitting, catching or tracking moving objects with the hand (Brenner & Smeets,
2011; Cesqui, Mezzetti, Lacquaniti, & d'Avella,
2015; van Donkelaar, Lee, & Gellman,
1994), presumably because of the beneficial effects of smooth pursuit on motion prediction (Bennett et al.,
2010; Spering et al.,
2011).