We can easily be aware and make use of time to accomplish everyday chores, skillful athletic performances, speech, and music, just to name a few. In actuality, however, we must solve this seemingly easy task of knowing time without any hardwired sense organs specifically evolved to sense physical energy corresponding to the concept of time itself. It is likely that we have conscious awareness of time as a result of integrative processes. As evidence for the integrative nature of time processing, our perception of time can be strikingly elusive in the influence of many factors, such as attention (Casini & Macar,
1997; Cicchini & Morrone,
2009; Herbst, van der Meer, & Busch,
2012; Lejeune,
1998; Seifried & Ulrich,
2011; Tse, Intriligator, Rivest, & Cavanagh,
2004), arousal (Stetson, Fiesta, & Eagleman,
2007), expectancy (Pariyadath & Eagleman,
2007,
2008; Schindel, Rowlands, & Arnold,
2011; Ulrich, Nitschke, & Rammsayer,
2006), order (Hellström,
1985), size (Ono & Kawahara,
2007; Ono & Kitazawa,
2009; Rammsayer & Verner,
2014), and a variety of sensory signals, including visual dynamics. One of the most representative time illusions related to visual dynamics is adaptation-induced time compression (Johnston, Arnold, & Nishida,
2006), which demonstrates that adaptation to rapid flicker reduces the subjective duration of a subsequently presented stimulus. Another striking example is illusory time dilation of dynamic visual events (Kanai, Paffen, Hogendoorn, & Verstraten,
2006; Kaneko & Murakami,
2009), which is the target of this study. In a variety of situations, moving or flickering visual stimuli appear to last longer than their stationary counterparts.