SKD has also been reported in normal observers, termed
physiologic SKD (Hudson & Wild,
1992; Osako, Osako, Hashimoto, & Okano,
1997; Safran & Glaser,
1980) as well as in those with visual loss arising from retinal or anterior visual pathway disease (rather than exclusively arising from higher cortical lesions; Casson, Osako, Johnson, & Hwang,
1991; Charlier, Defoort, Rouland, & Hache,
1989; Finkelstein & Johnson,
1989; Gandolfo, Rossi, Ermini, & Zinigirian,
1995; Safran & Glaser,
1980; Tsutsui, Ichihashi, & Kimura,
1984; Wedermeyer, Johnson, & Keltner,
1989; Yabuki, Sakai, Suzumura, Endo, & Matsuo,
1989; Zappia et al.,
1971). In normal observers, physiologic SKD has been defined as discordance between the position of the kinetic isopter of a fixed contrast stimulus found using a one-way method of limits (MoL) with the spatial position of a target when its threshold is measured using static perimetry, which has been taken to indicate greater relative sensitivity to a kinetic stimulus (Hudson & Wild,
1992; Osako et al.,
1997). Although both Riddoch's syndrome and physiologic SKD are hypothesized to reflect methodological differences between static and kinetic procedures, local adaptation and stimulus habituation due to repeated presentations at the same location in static perimetry when using custom test paradigms examining only a few test locations or spatial summation with a moving target (Hudson & Wild,
1992; Osako et al.,
1997), differing neural mechanisms behind the detection of static and motion stimuli have also been suggested (Safran & Glaser,
1980).