A liquid's optical material appearance can tell us many things about the liquid. For example, water is colorless and transparent, whereas milk is translucent; caramel and chocolate sauce have distinctive colors, whereas molten solder is lustrous. Because specific optical characteristics are associated with particular liquids, we could use the optical appearance—or low-level image correlates—to narrow down the range of expected behaviors of the liquid. In addition to the large amount of literature on the perception of surface color (see Foster,
2011, for a review), a growing body of research has investigated the estimation of optical properties such as gloss (Beck & Prazdny,
1981; Nishida & Shinya,
1998; Fleming, Dror, & Adelson,
2003; Motoyoshi, Nishida, Sharan, & Adelson,
2007; Ho, Landy, & Maloney,
2008; Kim, Marlow, & Anderson,
2012; see Chadwick & Kentridge,
2015, for a recent review), translucency (Fleming, Jensen, & Bülthoff,
2004; Fleming & Bülthoff,
2005; Xiao et al.,
2014), transparency (Fleming, Jäkel, & Maloney,
2011; Faul & Ekroll,
2012; Schlüter & Faul,
2014) and surface texture (Landy & Graham,
2004; Dong & Chantler,
2005; Emrith, Chantler, Green, Maloney, & Clarke,
2010; Liu, Dong, Cai, Qi, & Chantler,
2015). These findings suggest that human observers are generally very good at inferring optical material properties under a wide range of conditions, and thus it is plausible that observers could base judgments about liquids on such cues.