Eye tracking is an important research tool which measures the movements of the eyes. It is an established technique to investigate the comprehension and understanding of, for example, a text (Rayner, Chace, Slattery, & Ashby,
2009) or an image (Rayner,
2009). Research using eye tracking also includes clinical applications—for example, examination of eye-movement dysfunctions in individuals with schizophrenia (Flechtner, Steinacher, Sauer, & Mackert,
1997) or dyslexia (Eden, Stein, Wood, & Wood,
1994) and in the human vestibular system (Allison, Eizenman, & Cheung,
1996). The two most common types of events in eye-tracking data are
fixations and
saccades. A fixation is when the eye is more or less still and visual information is taken in. A saccade is instead a fast eye movement that redirects the eye from one position to the next. When the eye follows a smoothly moving target, the eye movement is called a
smooth pursuit. In order to see a moving object clearly during smooth pursuit, the object must be aligned with the direction of gaze. When the object is not perfectly followed by the eye, small corrective saccades are used to realign the direction of the gaze to that of the moving object. A smooth pursuit is divided into two stages: open-loop and closed-loop (Leigh & Zee,
2006, p. 219). The open-loop stage is the initial stage when the smooth pursuit is initiated by a movement of an object. The second, closed-loop stage is a feedback system where the velocity of the eye is controlled in order to keep the eye on the moving object. The upper limit for the velocity of a smooth pursuit movement in a natural task is above 100°/s (Hayhoe, McKinney, Chajka, & Pelz,
2012). No lower limit for smooth pursuit velocity seems to exist, and the pursuit system can operate in the same velocity range as fixational eye movements (Martins, Kowler, & Palmer,
1985).