We move our eyes about three times every second. With each eye movement the visual system receives a new retinal image and needs to identify and locate the items of interest. This processing might begin from a blank slate, as if seeing each image for the first time; alternatively, the carryover of information across the eye movement could provide computational benefits. However, numerous studies showed that the carryover of information is sparse. We are blind to most changes of an image across eye movements or other interruptions except for changes to previously attended items (Jensen, Yao, Street, & Simons,
2011; O'Regan, Rensink, & Clark,
1999; Simons & Ambinder,
2005). Based on this change blindness work, it would seem that most of the scene is processed anew after each eye movement. This would require some kind of mechanism to establish a correspondence between pre- and postsaccadic locations of the same object and allow visual stability. Early proposals as to how this stability was achieved assumed that the postsaccadic image was fused with a lingering presaccadic image. This fusion across multiple fixations formed a single, highly detailed, spatiotopic percept of the visual scene (e.g., Banks,
1983; Breitmeyer, Hoar, Randall, & Conte,
1984; Breitmeyer, Kropfl, & Julesz,
1982; Feldman,
1985; Jonides, Irwin, & Yantis,
1982; Trehub,
1977). For example, Wolf, Hauske, and Lupp (
1980) reported that a sinusoidal grating presented before a saccade could still be seen briefly after the saccade at the same spatial location even if it had been removed before the saccade landed. Jonides et al. (
1982) reported that partial matrices of dots were combined across eye movements. However, subsequent studies showed that both of these results were due to the phosphor persistence on the cathode ray tube (CRT) monitors rather to transsaccadic fusion (Irwin, Zacks, & Brown,
1990; Jonides, Irwin, & Yantis,
1983). In addition to these methodological issues, the perceptual fusion hypothesis then faced a series of studies that demonstrated a complete absence of transsaccadic fusion at the perceptual level. In particular, observers were unable to identify patterns when two halves are presented successively before and after a saccade (Bridgeman & Mayer,
1983; Irwin, Yantis, & Jonides,
1983; O'Regan & Levy-Schoen,
1983; Rayner & Pollatsek,
1983). This has commonly been taken as evidence that visual carryover across saccades does not occur.