Consistent with the theoretical framework advanced by de Groot, Gobet, Chase, and Simon, chess experts display a wide range of perceptual advantages that are predicted by chunking and template theory (for reviews, see Charness,
1992; de Groot & Gobet,
1996; Gobet & Charness,
2006; Reingold & Charness,
2005; Reingold & Sheridan,
2011). For example, as evidence that experts process larger visual patterns than novices, chess experts display higher proportions of fixations
between rather than
on chess pieces in comparison with less skilled players (Charness, Reingold, Pomplun, & Stampe,
2001; Reingold & Charness,
2005; Reingold, Charness, Pomplun, & Stampe,
2001; see also de Groot & Gobet,
1996; Jongman,
1968), and chess experts exhibit a larger
visual span than less skilled players when they are fixating on domain-related visual configurations (Reingold, Charness, Pomplun et al.,
2001). As well, chess experts can use their memory for chess configurations to rapidly focus on relevant information and to ignore irrelevant information (Bilalić, Langner, Erb, & Grodd,
2010; Bilalić, Turella, Campitelli, Erb, & Grodd,
2012; Charness et al.,
2001; de Groot & Gobet,
1996; Reingold & Charness,
2005; Sheridan & Reingold,
2014; Simon & Barenfeld,
1969; Tikhomirov & Poznyanskaya,
1966). Finally, neuroimaging studies have uncovered expert/novice differences in brain activation in regions associated with object and pattern recognition (Bilalić et al.,
2010,
2012; Bilalić, Kiesel, Pohl, Erb, & Grodd,
2011; Bilalić, Langner, Ulrich, & Grodd,
2011), and chess expertise modulates ERPs (Event-Related Potentials) to chess-related stimuli as early as 240 ms poststimulus (Wright, Gobet, Chassy, & Ramchandani,
2013). Taken together, these findings support the view that perceptual skill is a key aspect of expertise in chess.