The eye movement restriction introduced by Luu and Howe (
2015) allowed viewers to only use covert attention to track targets. Another recent study by Szinte, Carrasco, Cavanagh, and Rolfs (
2015) using a paradigm with apparent motion also showed that covert attention is shifted ahead of an attended target object. In other studies using apparent motion, Shiori, Cavanagh, Miyamoto, and Yaguchi (
2000) and Shiori, Yamamoto, Kageyama, and Yaguchi (
2002) showed that covert attention shifts along smoothly with an attended object, in a way predicting its future apparent location. For overt tracking, one study showed that top-down attentional processes allocate resources broadly ahead of an object during smooth pursuit (Khan, Lefèvre, Heinen, & Blohm,
2010), while other studies showed that overt attention was centered on tracked objects (Lovejoy, Fowler, & Krauzlis,
2009; Watamaniuk & Heinen,
2015). These examples are investigations of overt or covert attention in isolation. However, in many object tracking tasks, as well as in real-life situations, a mix of overt and covert attention may play a role (cf. Fehd & Seiffert,
2008). In the study presented here, we therefore investigate the relative contributions of overt and covert attention (Posner,
1980) with regard to anticipatory attention in object tracking tasks, first at the lowest possible tracking load in the form of single-object tracking (SOT), and later at a higher tracking load (but still low enough to expect anticipatory attention) in the form of a two-target MOT task. We use a similar probe detection paradigm as in Atsma et al. (
2012), because it allows us to map the relative allocation of attentional resources around tracked targets by presenting probes at specific locations relative to the target's movement direction. By definition, overt attention corresponds to target fixation, whereas covert attention corresponds to attending a target without fixation (Posner,
1980). Using this assumption, we combine the abovementioned paradigm with specific fixation instructions (and corresponding control), such that viewers have to fixate either the moving target or a fixation cross at the center of the display (
Experiment 1), or fixate one target while covertly tracking a second target (
Experiment 2). As a measure for anisotropy we compare probe detection rates at different angles relative to the target's movement direction. If eye fixation on a target is necessary for anticipation, we expect to see an anisotropic distribution of probe detection rates around overtly tracked targets rather than covertly tracked targets, with better detection ahead rather than behind the target (i.e., anticipatory). However, if attention anticipates movement regardless of where you look, we expect to see anisotropic distribution of probe detection rates around covertly tracked targets as well.